Skip to main content
Log in

Abstract

Collaborative learning has often been associated with the construction of a shared understanding of the situation at hand. The psycholinguistics mechanisms at work while establishing common grounds are the object of scientific controversy. We postulate that collaborative tasks require some level of mutual modelling, i.e. that each partner needs some model of what the other partners know/want/intend at a given time. We use the term “some model” to stress the fact that this model is not necessarily detailed or complete, but that we acquire some representations of the persons we interact with. The question we address is: Does the quality of the partner model depend upon the modeler’s ability to represent his or her partner? Upon the modelee’s ability to make his state clear to the modeler? Or rather, upon the quality of their interactions? We address this question by comparing the respective accuracies of the models built by different team members. We report on 5 experiments on collaborative problem solving or collaborative learning that vary in terms of tasks (how important it is to build an accurate model) and settings (how difficult it is to build an accurate model). In 4 studies, the accuracy of the model that A built about B was correlated with the accuracy of the model that B built about A, which seems to imply that the quality of interactions matters more than individual abilities when building mutual models. However, these findings do not rule out the fact that individual abilities also contribute to the quality of modelling process.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The data and statistical analyses of this study are available online: https://github.com/chili-epfl/mutual-modelling-emotions-study.

References

  • Anderson, A.H., O’Malley, C., Doherty-Sneddon, G., Langton, S., Newlands, A., Mullin, J., Fleming, A.M., & Van der Velden, J. (1997). The impact of VMC on collaborative problem solving: An analysis of task performance, communicative process, and user satisfaction., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, NJ, US, pp 133–155. Computers, cognition, and work.

  • Aronson, E., Blaney, N., & Stephan, C. (1978). Sikes J, The jigsaw classroom. Sage Publications, Snapp M.

  • Baker, M., Hansen, T., Joiner, R., & Traum, D. (1999). The role of grounding in collaborative learning tasks. Collaborative learning: Cognitive and computational approaches 31–63.

  • Blaye, A., & Light, P. (1995). Collaborative problem solving with HyperCard: the influence of peer interaction on planning and information handling strategies. In: Computer supported collaborative learning, Springer, pp 3–22.

  • Brennan, S.E. (1991). Conversation with and through computers. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 1(1), 67–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherubini, M., Van Der Pol, J., & Dillenbourg, P. (2005). In Grounding is not shared understanding: Distinguishing grounding at an utterance and knowledge level CONTEXT’05, the Fifth International and Interdisciplinary Conference on Modeling and Using Context (pp. 11–23).

  • Chi, M.T., Bassok, M., Lewis, M.W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive science, 13(2), 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H., & Brennan, S.E. (1991). Grounding in communication. Perspectives on socially shared cognition, 13(1991), 127–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H., & Marshall, C.R. (2002). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. Psycholinguistics: critical concepts in psychology 414.

  • Clark, H.H., & Schaefer, E.F. (1989). Contributing to discourse. Cognitive science, 13(2), 259–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillenbourg, P., & Hong, F. (2008). The mechanics of cscl macro scripts. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 3(1), 5–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillenbourg, P., & Traum, D. (2006). Sharing solutions: Persistence and grounding in multimodal collaborative problem solving. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15 (1), 121–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillenbourg, P., Baker, M.J., Blaye, A., & O’Malley, C. (1995). The evolution of research on collaborative learning. Learning in Humans and Machine: Towards an interdisciplinary learning science 189–211.

  • Doise, W., Mugny, G., & Perret-Clermont, A.N. (1975). Social interaction and the development of cognitive operations. European journal of social psychology, 5(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaver, W.W., Sellen, A., Heath, C., & Luff, P. (1993). One is not enough: Multiple views in a media space. In Proceedings of the INTERACT’93 and CHI’93 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 335–341).

  • Hutchins, E., & Palen, L. (1997). Constructing meaning from space, gesture, and speech. In Discourse, Tools and Reasoning (pp. 23–40): Springer.

  • Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., Bolger, N., & etal (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. The handbook of social psychology, 1(4), 233–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobbe, L., Weinberger, A., Dillenbourg, P., Harrer, A., Hämäläinen, R., Häkkinen, P., & Fischer, F. (2007). Specifying computer-supported collaboration scripts. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 2(2-3), 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L.S., Patterson, A.H., & Newcombe, N. (1981). Spatial representation and behavior across the life span. Academic Press.

  • Molinari, G., Sangin, M., Nüssli, M.A., & Dillenbourg, P. (2008). Effects of knowledge interdependence with the partner on visual and action transactivity in collaborative concept mapping. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on International Conference for the Learning Sciences - Volume 2 (pp. 91–98).

  • Moreland, R.L. (1999). Transactive memory: Learning who knows what in work groups and organizations. Shared Cognition in Organizations: The Management of Knowledge.

  • Nova, N., Girardin, F., & Dillenbourg, P. (2005). Location is not enough!: an empirical study of location-awareness in mobile collaboration. In IEEE International Workshop on Wireless and Mobile Technologies in Education (pp. 21–28).

  • Nova, N., Girardin, F., Molinari, G., & Dillenbourg, P. (2006). The underwhelming effects of automatic location-awareness on collaboration in a pervasive game. In Cooperative Systems Design: Seamless Integration of Artifacts and Conversations-Enhanced Concepts of Infrastructure for Communication (pp. 224–238).

  • Nova, N., Wehrle, T., Goslin, J., Bourquin, Y., & Dillenbourg, P. (2007). Collaboration in a multi-user game: impacts of an awareness tool on mutual modeling. Multimedia tools and Applications, 32(2), 161–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M.J., & Garrod, S. (2006). Alignment as the basis for successful communication. Research on Language and Computation, 4(2-3), 203–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J., & Teasley, S.D. (1995). The construction of shared knowledge in collaborative problem solving. In Computer supported collaborative learning (pp. 69–97): Springer.

  • Sangin, M., Molinari, G., Nüssli, M.A., & Dillenbourg, P. (2008). How learners use awareness cues about their peer’s knowledge?: insights from synchronized eye-tracking data. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on International conference for the learning sciences-Volume 2, International Society of the Learning Sciences (pp. 287–294).

  • Schober, M.F. (1993). Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition, 47 (1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D.L. (1995). The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(3), 321–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slugoski, B.R., Lalljee, M., Lamb, R., & Ginsburg, G.P. (1993). Attribution in conversational context: Effect of mutual knowledge on explanation-giving. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23(3), 219–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, G. (2007). Meaning making in CSCL: Conditions and preconditions for cognitive processes by groups. In Proceedings of the 8th iternational conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (pp. 652–661).

  • Suthers, D.D. (2006). Technology affordances for intersubjective meaning making: A research agenda for cscl. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 1(3), 315–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Traum, D., & Dillenbourg, P. (1996). Miscommunication in multi-modal collaboration. In AAAI Workshop on Detecting, Repairing, and Preventing Human–Machine Miscommunication (pp. 37–46).

  • Webb, N.M. (1991). Task-related verbal interaction and mathematics learning in small groups. Journal for research in mathematics education 366–389.

  • Wegner, D.M. (1987). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In Theories of Group Behavior (pp. 185–208): Springer.

Download references

Acknowledgments

The experiments were developed with the help of Mirweis Sangin, René Glaus, Patrick Jermann, Fabien Girardin, Marc-Antoine Nüssli, Thomas Werhle, Yvan Bourquin and Jeremy Goslin. We also thank Kshitij Sharma and Łukasz Kidziński for their help with data analysis. The main funding has been provided from a NSF Grant grant #102511-106940.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Séverin Lemaignan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dillenbourg, P., Lemaignan, S., Sangin, M. et al. The symmetry of partner modelling. Intern. J. Comput.-Support. Collab. Learn 11, 227–253 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-016-9235-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-016-9235-5

Keywords

Navigation