Introduction

Psychologists have argued that people modify their attitudes and behavior when they think that others are observing them (Morin 2004, 2011). Once people become cognizant that their demeanor, appearance, or actions are monitored in public, a self-awareness process begins. Self-awareness triggers a complex cognitive process of self-scrutiny and a heightened construction of one’s own identity (Wicklund 1975). Evidence of these phenomena is ubiquitous and dates back more than five decades (Duval and Wicklund 1972). More importantly, it is generally agreed that people who are aware of being observed adapt their attitudes and behavior to match what they perceive to be more “acceptable social norms” (Munger and Harris 1989; Sproull et al. 1996) or “socially desirable responses” (Jones and Nisbett 1971; Paulhus 1988).

We examine the application of this line of research to body-worn cameras (BWCs). BWCs are used to videotape interactions between police officers and members of the public in an attempt to “cool down” these encounters. Because BWCs work as third-party observers, it is assumed that they elicit the above-mentioned psychosocial processes (Ariel et al. 2015). Although evidence on their effectiveness is beginning to build up, the intended and unintended consequences of using this technology in policing remain unclear (Cubitt et al. 2017, Maskaly et al. 2017, Lum et al. 2015; White and Coldren 2017). The focus in this study is on assaults against police officers, while trying to understand under what conditions BWCs—or self-awareness of being observed—lead to socially desirable behaviors.

Our larger aim is to illustrate the benefits of self-awareness theory in sociology, particularly for deterrence and social control scholars. We argue that these psychosocial mechanisms provide the missing link between real-world deterrence stimuli and changes in both attitudes and behavior; this concept has largely been left to crude assumptions in the deterrence literature. More profoundly, whereas deterrence per se is difficult to quantify and observe directly, self-awareness can and has been experimentally induced. Therefore, self-awareness is not only a phenomenological concept but is also susceptible to manipulation to the point that society can utilize this body of knowledge in the regulation of behavior. BWCs are one such stimuli.

The paper is structured as follows. First, we review relevant literature on self-awareness, being observed, and consequent socially desirable responses. We then discuss how these are potentially linked to deterrence theory. We next present the state of the art of the evidence on BWCs, including the possible moderating factors that constrain or exacerbate the effect of BWCs in police–public encounters. We subsequently discuss the hypothesized effect of BWCs on assaults against police officers. The research design and analytical strategy used to test these hypotheses are described in the methods and data section, followed by an exposition of the results. Based on the use of an experimental design across ten participating police departments, we defend the view that our causal estimates are robust. We present a complex story about the effect of BWCs on assaults on officers. We found a significant before–after reduction in assaults, whereas the between-group comparisons yielded a perverse result: greater odds of assaults against officers who use BWCs than the odds under control conditions. Subgroup analyses are used to explain these paradoxical effects. We suggest that the underlying mechanism is police discretion: weak discretion in the use of BWCs and the application of police force is linked to more assaults. It seems that BWCs may cause “over-deterrence,” and officers may become hesitant to apply necessary force, which in turn results in more assaults. Instead of using force to subdue hostile suspects following a split-second decision, some officers are reluctant to use force due to fear of reprimands for using any type of force inappropriately. This phenomenon represents a “dark side” of self-awareness that was previously unknown.

Theoretical framework

Self-awareness

Self-awareness refers to the process of focusing attention inward toward the self (Mead 1934), or the capacity of becoming the object of one’s own attention. One becomes self-aware when one reflects on the experience of perceiving and processing stimuli. Intuitively, many of us are familiar with this feeling; merely being in front of an audience induces self-awareness (Buss 1980). However, self-awareness represents a complex multidimensional phenomenon that comprises many domains and corollaries (Eilan et al. 1995; Morin 2011; Wicklund 1975).Footnote 1

Duval and Wicklund (1972) showed that self-awareness could be experimentally induced. Their findings demonstrated that self-awareness is not only a phenomenological concept but also a psychosocial (as well as neurological) process that society can utilize in the regulation of behavior. We now know that a wide range of stimuli can trigger self-awareness (Butterworth 1992, 1995; Cole and Stewart 1999). Specific “self-focusing stimuli” such as cameras, audiences, recordings, and mirrors have been tested in controlled settings to stimulate self-awareness (Buss 1980; Carver and Scheier 1978; Davis and Brock 1975; Davis and Franzoi 1991; Geller and Shaver 1976; Morin 2002). For example, with mirrors, the reflection provides a source of feedback that allows the individual to adjust her or his presentation to convey a desired image to those around her or him (Gervais and Norenzayan 2012). Collectively, studies have shown that by inducing this process of self-awareness, we become aware of our “self-domains” (e.g., self-criticism, self-efficacy) as well as our “public self-dimensions,” such as our own behaviors, mannerisms, and appearance (Duval and Wicklund 1972). A substantial body of research supports the basic concept in social psychology that being self-aware triggers a cognitive process of self-scrutiny and a heightened construction of one’s identity (see review in Froming et al. 1982).

The evidence on the effects of increased self-awareness on behavior is broad and extensive. A prominent and objective outcome of self-awareness is the adaptation of behavior (Chartrand and Bargh 1999; Dzieweczynski et al. 2006; Heine et al. 2008; Jones and Nisbett 1971; Morin 2005). Some of the behavioral manifestations found in previous research are avoidance (Moskalenko and Heine 2003), self-harm (Baumeister 1990; Morin and Craig 2000), increased emotional intensity (Silvia 2002), and—particularly pertinent to our research—self-regulation (Bandura 1978; Carver and Scheier 2004; Kanfer 1977). Self-regulation includes “altering one’s behavior, resisting temptation, changing one’s mood, [and] selecting a response from various options” (Morin 2011: 814). Note that these are dimensions within the self that are perceived as requiring control.

When thinking about how increased self-awareness leads to socially desirable responses, research findings generally agree that individuals react in socially desirable ways to even the slightest cues indicating that someone may be watching (Bateson et al. 2006; Boyd et al. 2010; Burnham and Johnson 2005; Haley and Fessler 2005; but cf. Northover et al. 2017). Therefore, self-awareness of being observed heightens the need to cooperate with rules (Barclay 2004; Milinski et al. 2002; Wedekind and Braithwaite 2002).

Most sociologists should feel at home with real-world displays of the process that leads to socially desirable behavior (i.e., self-awareness of being observed) because, thus far, we have described the psychosocial mechanisms behind deterrence theory (Jervis 1989; Tibbetts 2013; for more recent explorations of moderators of being observed and socially desirable behavior, see Pfattheicher 2015; Pfattheicher and Keller 2015). Knowing with sufficient certainty that our behavior is being observed or judged affects social cognitive processes that make us comply with codes of desirable behavior. These codes are often found in law and regulations or in the socially desirable responses in social interactions mentioned previously (Ariel et al. 2015). Deterrence theory offers the requirement of a heightened level of appreciation that not only is someone observing our behavior but that the observing entity will punish us for transgressions. At a high level of abstraction, there is ample evidence that the perceived certainty of punishment is causally associated with less rule-breaking (Ariel and Partridge 2016; Bushway and Reuter 2008; Lochner 2003; Loughran et al. 2012; McCord and Conway 2002; Nagin 2013a; Nagin 2013b; Paternoster 2010; Pratt et al. 2006; but cf. Berk and MacDonald 2010; Tonry 2008; Wikström and Treiber 2007). More than the severity of sanctions and probably more than the celerity of sanctions (Von Hirsch et al. 1999), increasing the perceived likelihood of being caught is inversely linked to the likelihood of breaking rules. This “certainty effect” carries wide probabilities over a range of settings in which society attempts deterrence (Nagin 2013a).

Body-worn cameras

One stimulus that has recently been proposed to encourage self-awareness and lead to socially desirable behavior is police BWCs. The police have enthusiastically endorsed BWCs, as have politicians, civil rights advocates and the public (Obama 2015). This interest has generated a growing multibillion-dollar industry (Sutherland and Ariel 2016). BWCs are small audio–video recorders that are “mounted” on police officers. They have two overarching goals. On the one hand, by recording police–public interactions from the officer’s perspective, BWCs are believed to prevent escalations or new crimes from occurring (White 2014), as well as protect officers from violating rules of conduct. On the other hand, they may assist in bringing offenders to justice (Morrow et al. 2016). Once suspects become (or are made) aware of the cameras, and officers are reminded about the cameras, a deterrent effect is hypothesized for those present in the encounter: “Look out! You are on camera” (Jennings et al. 2015; Ready and Young 2015).

Theoretically, the deterrence message may work to change behavior in different ways. For example, the camera and verbal prompt could lead to increased self-awareness for both officers and suspects, which in turn leads to a greater level of rational–deliberative action (including calculations of risks of sanction threat/punishment), increasing self-regulation/behavioral inhibition, and perhaps a closer adherence to rules of conduct (Ariel et al. 2015: 516 Ariel 2016). Since the risk of apprehension for misconduct, hostile, or criminal behavior is elevated to near certainty (when the cameras are turned on), both parties are believed to “cool off” (Ariel 2016a). The “belligerent” suspect is expected to become less resistant, and the “thin-skinned” police officer is expected to respond more professionally, demonstrating fairness, dignity, and respect and using proportional force only when necessary (Bottoms and Tankebe 2012; Tyler 1990; Tyler and Lind 1992). Conversely, it may be that the cameras primarily nudge the suspect toward being more deliberative, and that in turn changes the overall tenor of the interaction, and it is speculation as to whether self-regulation does matter. At the same time, the speed with which such interactions take place, and the very fact they are reciprocal in nature, means that existing research is unable to answer questions about who is more affected by the cameras.

Evidence of BWCs’ effectiveness has surfaced only in the last few years. A recent literature search of the available evidence conducted by Lum et al. (2015) reported on 12 published studies of BWCs. The review concluded that there were too few studies to draw firm conclusions, but that there were some emerging areas relating to the possible effects of cameras on complaints, accountability, stop and frisk, arrest, and use of force (to name but a few).Footnote 2

Moderating factors

Since the Lum et al. (2015) review, additional research has been published that tries to explain the conditions under which BWCs work. The self-awareness effect of BWCs is conditional on the extent to which the cameras are activated (Hedberg et al. 2017; Taylor 2016; Young and Ready 2016), which is unsurprising: if officers choose to not turn the cameras on, the mechanism that causes self-awareness and the cognitive process of self-scrutiny will not materialize. The prerequisite condition is ensuring that officers activate the cameras. However, a major contributing factor to patterns of police officers’ attitudes and behaviors, including the use of force, complaints against the police, or injuries in the line of duty, is the institutional subculture (Blau and Blau 1982; Galliher 1971; Herbert 1998; Sherman 1980; Waddington 1999). Police “in-group” cohesion is preserved by styles with a “common vision that becomes part of each officer’s mindset of how to handle everyday aspects of policing” (Terrill 2001: 20). This means that officers tend to react similarly to given situations because of organizational operating procedures, incentives, and top-down managerial and bottom-up behavioral models (Sherman 1980; Wilson 1968, Tankebe 2016). An ethos and mutual conceptualization of reality thus develops in any police department that may include the tacit approval of certain actions that are otherwise inappropriate (Brown 1998; Skolnick 2008).

Police subculture is relevant to BWC research because so-called “big-brother little nudges”, intrusions into day-to-day operations, are believed to cause wide departmental effects. Ariel et al. (2016a) have reported direct evidence that repeated and systematic exposure to a stimulus that elicits deterrence can increase accountability, even when the stimulus is removed. Through repeated exposure to the surveillance effect of the cameras, police officers learn what normative or appropriate reactions are, even when they are no longer under surveillance (during control conditions). This may be true for officers who once wore BWCs and no longer wear them (through the process of random assignment) or officers in the department who did not take part in the experiment (e.g., neighborhood police teams, special victim support units). Ariel et al. (2016b) argued that BWCs affect entire police departments through a process they labeled “contagious accountability.” Everyone is affected by the BWCs, even when the cameras are not in use; thus, the subculture has become less aggressive and more compliant with procedures.

Use of BWCs to reduce assaults against police officers

It is logical to assume that when officers use BWCs, they should be assaulted less frequently. If BWCs can “cool off” aggressive encounters (Ariel et al. 2015; Jennings et al. 2015; Sutherland 2017), they are also likely to reduce the likelihood of assaults against the police. An individual who assaults an officer may be shot (in the US, at least), but the individual will almost certainly be punished more severely if the event can be proved in court. Therefore, rational suspects would avoid assaulting officers when the likelihood of apprehension for assaulting an officer is high (Whichard and Felson 2016). This is the case for BWCs: the probability of being caught assaulting a police officer is substantially increased because a camera pointing at the transgressor provides unequivocal evidence of the assault. The assumption is therefore that reasonable offenders will not want to be caught and subsequently punished for misbehavior (Margarita 1980; Ojedokun 2014). In turn, the officer is also less likely to act in ways that would provoke the suspect, which in turn will cause the officer to respond in a nonprofessional way. As both parties become aware that the camera is observing them, the elevated self-regulation mechanism is hypothesized to reduce the need to apply forceful police responses. Consequently, assaults on officers should be minimized.

Before testing this hypothesis, it is important to contextualize the scope, prevalence, and taxonomy of assaults against the police, on which, collectively, the literature seems somewhat unclear (Bierie 2015). Many studies suggest that approximately 10% of officers are injured during use-of-force incidents (Henriquez 1999; Kaminski 2004; Kaminski et al. 2004; Smith and Petrocelli 2002; Smith et al. 2007), but some studies have observed rates of up to 38% (Alpert and MacDonald 2001; Kaminski and Sorensen 1995). Recorded injuries are relatively minor in terms of severity (Alpert et al. 2000; Brandl 1996; Brandl and Stroshine 2003; Kaminski 2004). The most common cause of injury to officers during arrest is when officers attempt to subdue a suspect with bodily force (punching, kicking, take-downs, wrestling and joint locks); these techniques account for most recorded injuries (Kaminski and Sorensen 1995; Meyer 1992; Smith and Petrocelli 2002). Kaminski and Sorensen (1995) found that officers are more likely to suffer an injury during an incidence of use of force if they are employing levels of force at the lower end of the “force continuum” (Garner et al. 1995), such as “hands-on tactics” that require close contact with a suspect (see also Fryer 2016).

Methods and data

Settings

Ten police departments adopted the same experimental protocol to test the effect of BWCs on assaults against the police under controlled conditions. Although a convenience sample of police departments should not be construed as representative of the entire population of police departments, the results from a ten-site design in six jurisdictions and two English-speaking countries provide a robust framework for testing the effect of self-awareness on assaults against the police on the one hand, or aggression by the police on the other. Table 1 provides the basic descriptors of each site (two separate geographic areas were included in the trial; hence, there is a greater number of trials than police forces). The total number of residents policed across the ten jurisdictions that took part in this study was above 2 million. During the study period, a total of 2122 officers made more than 15,600 arrests.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for the ten participating sites

Random assignment

Beyond large-scale cluster-randomized designs, randomizing shifts is the most practical approach to implementing BWC trials with the police because even small forces can leverage a large sample size.Footnote 3 Each study was a two-armed trial that randomly assigned officer shifts to either experimental (with cameras) or control (no cameras) conditions on a weekly basis using the Cambridge Randomizer (Ariel et al. 2012). This means that the shift is the unit of analysis, not individual officers as with other BWC studies. Weekly sequences of treatment and control shift were communicated to the patrol officers. Shift randomization resulted in 4264 shifts being assigned (M = 609.1; SD = 264.0 per site), with equal allocations of day and night shifts, including days of the week (see Table 2). No differences were observed between the treatment and control conditions in terms of the distribution of shifts.Footnote 4

Table 2 Trial measures by treatment allocation within each participating site

Treatment and control conditions

The pre-published protocol agreed upon with each police department stated that officers on “camera on” shifts had to keep the cameras on during their entire shift (typically 8–12 h) and inform members of the public during any encounter that they were wearing a camera that was recording their interaction. This means that the intervention consisted of {camera + notification} (on the importance of the interaction between these elements, see Sunstein and Thaler 2008; Sunstein 2014). The trial design meant that officers were not supposed to exercise discretion in deciding when cameras were turned on. The cameras were assigned to be on throughout their shift with every interaction with members of the public. However, in some instances officers did not record using devices between jobs (e.g., when traveling between calls for service) and while on rest-breaks. The only pre-agreed exceptions to interactions being recorded was when officers responded to specific types of incidents, as pre-agreed with senior staff in each force (e.g., when conversing with informants, serious sexual assaults, or major public events). Where officers were not recording between jobs, many devices have a “pre-event buffer” that continuously captures up to 2 min before officers press “record” on the device, meaning that the lead-up to a response should also be captured.

Treatment fidelity and manipulation checks

Maintaining consistent treatment integrity across several sites is a challenge in experimental criminology (see MacKenzie et al. 2013; de Brito 2017; Slothower et al. 2015; Weisburd and Taxman 2000). To address risks to fidelity, each trial was managed by a local researcher (see Drover and Ariel 2015). Routinely collected metadata captured by the BWCs, such as how much data had been recorded, along with date and time stamps, were also used to measure treatment integrity (see Table 3). Cross-tabulating between the date and time of evidence uploaded from the BWCs and random assignment provides a direct measure of manipulation checks. Any footage recorded during control shifts was considered a violation of the protocol—that is, a sign of noncompliance. A control shift with at least 1 minute of recorded footage was considered a breach of protocol, as BWCs should not have been taken out of the police station during control shifts. Conversely, when cameras recorded 0 minutes of data during a shift, this was an indication that the camera had been switched off or unused during the shift. This scenario was considered a violation of the experimental protocol as well, as it is unreasonable that any response officer on duty would have no recordable interaction. These are “Empirical Checks” of the manipulations, of which an example can be found in Supplementary Materials.

Table 3 Breakdown of sites based on treatment integrity

In addition, there were explicit circumstances where treatment integrity was compromised: when the police department openly announced the breakdown, a few months into the experiment. This means that officers in these experimental sites initially followed protocol, but then were granted discretion to wear, use, and record incidents, despite the experimental protocol. The protocol dictated that every encounter was to be videotaped as soon as the officer interacted with the suspect, witness, or victim during treatment shifts, but never to be used during control shifts.Footnote 5 However, some time after the trial commenced, some departments intentionally violated the protocol in some ways. This announcement/noncompliance took two forms: first, when the police department announced that officers were allowed to use the devices during both control shifts as well as treatment shifts, as they deemed fit. This occurred approximately 90 days into the trials. These are sites that can be characterized as lacking treatment integrity, because there were de facto no differences between treatment and control conditions shortly after the experiment commenced, in terms of the application of the manipulation. The rationale given for this breakdown was in the context of operational needs: when the sponsoring officer has taken the view that the initial testing period was sufficient for him/her to make a decision to deploy the devices as business-as-usual. We refer to these circumstances as “police-announced breakdown.”

A second type of noncompliance announcement characterized other departments: full compliance with the protocol in terms of control conditions, but failure to comply with the terms of treatment conditions. Despite the evidence against this policy (see Hedberg et al. 2017; Roy 2014), some departments gave officers the discretion to use BWCs how and when they deemed fit during treatment condition—that is, which interactions were worthy of recording, at which point during the police–public interaction to turn the cameras on or off, and whether or not to announce the recording of the interaction. Two major reasons for this decision were technological and/or financial: the battery life of the camera did not allow recording of every interaction, and the cost of uploading data, and the storage of the data exceeded the department’s budgetary constraints. At the same time, officers were instructed and subsequently followed the protocol in terms of control settings: devices were not to be used during “cameras off” shifts. Failing that, these circumstances can be seen as “Police-announced breakdown in Treatment Group.”

Outcome variables

We were granted access to official records of assaults on officers through a data-sharing agreement with each department. Because not all sites completed a 12-month trial, the assault counts were annualized for comparability purposes (Table 1). To clarify, assaults were defined as reports by the officers themselves before any internal investigation into the incident had been made. Officers can report assault for a wide range of reasons, including physical injuries and verbal abuse. Data-sharing issues meant that we were not given access to the types of assault but only to the total number of assaults during each shift (a point we return to in the discussion).

Statistical procedure

We used the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2 software (CMA) to synthesize the between-group results from the trials and present the overall results as a function of rates of assaults against the police per 1000 arrests. Because each trial used the same design and outcomes, it was appropriate to combine and report them as a prospective meta-analysis (Lipsey and Wilson 2001). The data inputted into CMA consisted of the rates of assaults per 1000 arrests in (i) control shifts and (ii) treatment shifts. We converted these rates into odds ratios to compare treatment and control conditions across all sites, and used the corresponding 95% confidence intervals as a measure of the reliability of the estimation procedure (Cohen 1988).

We verified the consistency of the meta-analytical results by measuring the influence of individual sites on the summary estimate using the “one study removed” technique (Higgins and Thompson 2002). This test allowed us to investigate sources of heterogeneity. Procedurally, this means that we ran the meta-analysis multiple times, each with a different single site removed. This procedure allowed us to detect whether any one site unduly influenced the observed effect.

We also conducted subgroup analyses based on an officer’s discretion to use BWCs because the decision to activate the camera has been suggested by previous studies to affect the use of force outcomes. Table 3 reports on compliance of each site. As shown, three compliance subgroups emerged: sites in which compliance was high (n = 3), sites in which compliance broke down (n = 4), and sites in which the police maintained compliance during control conditions, but failed to follow experimental protocol during treatment shifts only (n = 3). Put differently, four sites gave officers discretion on when and where BWCs should be used. Three sites stripped officers of their discretion completely, but were deemed as “high compliance” sites. Three more sites adhered to the protocol for control conditions, but allowed officers discretion as to when BWCs should be used in treatment cases, including at what point during the interaction BWCs were turned on in these treatment cases.

Finally, we also estimated treatment effects on a pre-test/post-test basis in each site. Although this is viewed as weak(er) evidence (Farrington et al. 2002), the exact timing of a policy’s implementation can be viewed as a natural experiment. Coupled with the multiple sites, jurisdictions, and timing of tests, the sum of this approach offers, in our view, useful evidence on potential causal effects. To assess pre–post differences we used a nonparametric analog to paired t-tests, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. This test is appropriate for paired comparisons when examining nominal variables when the differences are non-normally distributed and the overall number of pairs is relatively small (McDonald 2014:186–189). We also applied bootstrapping based on 1000 bootstrap replicates. Bootstrapping (Efron 1992, Hall and Wilson 1991) does not require distributional assumptions (e.g., normally distributed errors). Bootstraps also provide more accurate inferences, particularly when the sample size is small. We then used two-tailed Monte Carlo simulations based on 10,000 sampled tables with 99% confidence intervals to further validate our results under stricter statistical conditions (Mooney 1997; see also application in Ratcliffe 2005).

Results

Between-group experimental analyses

We detected a backfiring effect of BWCs in terms of assaults on police officers in the line of duty, rather than a reduction in these incidents (Fig. 1). A total of 394 assaults per 1000 arrests occurred during 3637 treatment shifts (M = 39.35, SD = 17.89) compared with 284 assaults per 1000 arrests during 3697 control shifts (M = 28.38; SD = 15.99).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Assaults on officer rates per 1000 arrests: treatment vs control conditions

Our meta-analysis of the data (see forest plot in Fig. 2) indicates that the odds of an assault against an officer during treatment conditions were between 15% to 62% greater than the odds of an assault against an officer during control conditions (average OR = 1.368; 95% CI 1.152, 1.624; p < 0.001).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Odds ratios of assaults against police officers (per 1000 arrests)

Based on our meta-analysis (Fig. 2), the directionality and relative magnitude of each point estimate in eight out of ten sites was the same (i.e., the same findings emerged at each site). Two sites, however—B and D (from the same jurisdiction)—deviated from the magnitude of the other sites [(OR = 43.352; 95% CI 2.584, 727.216; p < 0.01); (OR = 17.087; 95% CI 8.687, 33.608; p < 0.001) respectively]. These extreme estimates are not surprising given the fact that assaults against the police were 18 versus 0 and 78 versus 13 respectively, with a relatively small number of officers in these sites as well (Table 1). The extreme values in the two sites led to significant heterogeneity (Q = 74.42; p < 0.001; I2 = 79.4%). Removing these two sites removed the heterogeneity (Q = 10.82; p < 0.10; I2 = 35.31%) but also reduced the estimated effect size. Without the outlying sites, the odds of an assault against officers with cameras present ranged from − 5.1% to 35.4% compared to the odds under control conditions.

The “one study removed” sensitivity analysis did not change the directionality of the findings with respect to assaults against officers per 1000 arrests (Fig. 3). The point estimates of effect size ranged from a 15.0% to 48.1% increase in the odds of an assault against an officer per 1000 arrests with BWCs compared to the odds during control conditions. However, one site, a small and outlying site D, had a noticeable effect on the magnitude of the result. The removal of this study led to a relative change in the overall estimate of 15.9% (95% CI 0.963, 1.373).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Sensitivity analysis (one study removed): odds ratios of assaults against police officers (per 1000 arrests)

Fig. 4 below presents the subgroup analyses based on officers’ discretion. As shown, at sites where officers’ discretion regarding active cameras was high (n = 4), the odds of assaults against officers wearing BWCs were 37% higher than the odds of assaults in control conditions (OR = 1.365; 95% CI 1.066, 1.749). However, this backfiring treatment effect was substantially more pronounced in low-discretion sites (i.e., where officers followed the research protocol). The odds of assaults against officers under treatment conditions were 157% greater than under control conditions (OR = 2.565; 95% CI 1.792, 3.672).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Subgroup analysis: effect C on officers’ discretion of BWCs based on officers' discretion

At sites that allowed officers discretion regarding when and how to turn on BWCs during treatment conditions but did not allow officers to use the devices during control conditions (n = 3), a nonsignificant 16% reduction in the odds of assaults compared to control conditions (OR = .835, 95% CI .607, 1.149) was observed. Subgrouping the sites based on discretion removed the significant heterogeneity in this latter subgroup (Q = 2.74; p > 0.10) as well as in the high-discretion subgroup (Q = 2.34; p > 0.10) but not in the low-discretion subgroup (Q = 48.28; p < 0.01).

Pre–post analyses

Fig. 5 presents the counts of assaults across the ten experimental sites on a before–after basis. As shown, we counted 1418 assaults on officers in the 12 months prior to the study period and 547 assaults on officers during the study period (M = 141.8; SD = 117.2), or 1.21 assaults per officer. The number of assaults on officers decreased in the post-treatment period (M = 54.7 SD = 40.4), or 0.42 assaults per officer. This number indicates an overall reduction of 61% in the incidence of assaults. Wilcoxon signed-rank test indicated that post-test complaints were statistically significantly lower than the pre-test complaints (Z = −2.040; p = 0.041). The Monte Carlo simulations (99% CI .037, .047) as well as the bootstrapping approach (95% CI 28.3, 154.49) both yielded statistically significant differences between the pre and post periods.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Assaults on officers counts: before (n = 1418) to after (n = 547) comparisons in ten experiments

Discussion and conclusions

The emerging research evidence suggests that BWCs demarcate a shift in modern policing. Across thousands of police shifts in different parts of the world, over a long period of time and with millions of residents, BWCs have been found to (a) reduce complaints against officers to nearly zero (93% average reduction, (b) reduce the use of force by up to 50%, and (c) as shown here, reduce overall assaults against officers by nearly two-thirds on the before–after comparisons across most sites (Ariel et al. 2015, 2016a, b; Jennings et al. 2015). Arrest rates have also decreased by nearly one-fifth, and even cooperation with the police has increased as a result of the use of BWCs in policing (Ariel 2016a, b). One can make the argument that no single intervention in the history of policing can claim such substantial, evidenced effects in different jurisdictions (Alpert and Dunham 1997; Bayley 1977; Emsley 2009; Newburn 2005; Reiner 2010; Sherman 1978; Walker 1977; Walker and Katz 2012).

We contextualize the present study within the framework of self-awareness of being observed, which dates back to the work of George Mean on Mind, Self and Society (1934; see also Buss 1980; Duval and Wicklund 1972; Eilan et al. 1995; Morin 2002, 2011; Wicklund 1975). By doing so, this study continues the thread of scholastic work in social psychology and the psychology of the self as a way to explain how the mere presence of a third party in our physical world causes us to modify our perceptions, our motivations, and, ultimately, our behavior. Self-awareness triggers a cognitive process of self-scrutiny and a heightened construction of one’s own identity, which translates into behavior that is more compliant. In our case, we show that instead of the physical presence of a live observer, a small camera affixed to a shirt pocket causes dramatic changes.

However, there are questions about this evidence and the wider applicability of the results reported in these (and other) BWC studies. First, without a theoretical grounding, BWC experiments remain “black boxes” that tell us nothing about how effects are brought about. Second, to what extent can we generalize these findings beyond policing to the human condition and sociological theory more broadly? Below, we discuss such corollaries.

Theoretical implications

The effects of self-awareness on behavior provide a robust explanation for the processes behind deterrence theory as a bridge between psychology and sociology. Sociologists, and particularly criminologists, usually describe the process of deterrence from stimuli (e.g., more visible police, harsher sentences) and their aftermath (“less crime” or “more compliance”; Nagin 2013a; Nagin et al. 2015). Yet, at least in terms of a formal control apparatus, the story has mostly been rather crude: when the intervention appears, some behavioral modification is then observed (Braga et al. 2012, 2014). Precisely how the independent variables affect the dependent variables (i.e., less crime) is unclear (however, cf. Loughran et al. 2012). Self-awareness theory provides a mechanism that fits with the observed. It explains why, on whom, and under which conditions these causal links are created. It illustrates how the social world, physical stimuli, and the self interact (see Fig. 3 in Morin 2004).

Furthermore, focusing on these psychosocial processes offers an approach that is, above all, falsifiable. Deterrence, per se, is an abstract term that is difficult to quantify or measure directly (see discussion in Ariel et al. 2016a). However, more than five decades of research strongly shows that self-awareness can be experimentally induced through a clearly defined apparatus, such as an audience, recording, mirrors, or cameras (Buss 1980; Butterworth 1992, 1995; Carver and Scheier 1978; Cole and Stewart 1999; Davies 2005; Duval and Wicklund 1972; Geller and Shaver 1976; Morin 2002). Recent research even suggests that we can “view” these processes at the neurocognitive level (Cocchini et al. 2009; Dimaggio et al. 2008; Rameson and Lieberman 2009). Having the capacity to measure the treatment effect under controlled conditions is appealing to empiricists. Therefore, beyond deterrence scholars, we suspect that social control as well as rational choice theorists would find merit in the testability of these dimensions as they develop these theories in the future.

“Over-deterrence”

The evidence presented here show strong before–after effects across the various testing sites: the number of assaults on officers was significantly reduced by 61%, with a reliable downward trend in eight out of ten departments. At the same time, our between-group comparisons exhibit an increase rather than a decrease in assaults when comparing treatment with control shifts. Our sensitivity analysis suggests that the between-group effect size remains broadly consistent, usually between a 35% and 48% increase in the odds of assaults against officers per 1000 arrests in treatment conditions compared to the odds in control conditions. Therefore, this multisite study provides evidence of what Merton (1936) referred to as a “perverse effect”. Police departments as a whole experienced significantly fewer assaults—that is, all officers employed in these departments, with or without BWCs, experienced a lower likelihood of assault compared to the period prior to the experiment. At the same time, comparing treatment to control shifts actually showed that self-reported assaults were more likely when cameras were present.

Policing often requires the use of force to subdue aggressive criminals. Under these circumstances, officers must apply force not only to get the job done but also to protect themselves (see review in Friedrich 1980 as well as Sherman 1980, but see a critique in Skolnick and Fyfe 1993). However, we show that by becoming self-aware to a third-party observer such as BWCs, adverse results can emerge. Fig. 4 illustrates that as the degree of discretion decreases, the between-group odds of assaults against officers increases. These are police departments in which officers are fully cognizant that their interactions with the public are documented and that there is a strong likelihood of apprehension for rule violations. In the language of deterrence theory, “sanction threats” (see Ariel and Partridge 2016) were at their peak, and in the language of self-awareness theory, BWCs may have triggered a strong cognitive process of self-scrutiny (i.e., “am I doing the right thing right now?”). BWCs caused a significant reduction in the application of force (Ariel et al. 2016b). This seems like a desired consequence. At the same time, it seems that officers may have been “over-deterred” from applying force when force was in fact needed (Ariel 2017). In certain circumstances, some force is legitimate and, in fact, required. Deterrence was so strong that officers were reluctant to apply the force that was needed to stop force from being applied against them. These are undesired consequences.

We explain officers’ over-deterrence in two (though not necessarily mutually exclusive) ways.

  1. (1)

    Politeness as weakness. The first line of reasoning suggests that so-called “socially desirable responses” from officers are expected to include politeness and a procedural fairness approach. Some suspects may have interpreted this approach as weakness. As summarized by Kiesling (2007: 667), “Politeness is seen as powerless…men might see politeness as weakness.” In other words, while following the protocol strictly (i.e., weak discretion), officers conveyed a message to offenders that they were somehow vulnerable. As a result, officers may have been more likely to be viewed by suspects as ineffective sanction threats, submissive, or feeble. Against some types of criminals, this perceived weakness translated into physical action in the form of assaults against officers.

    Furthermore, it is likely that BWCs cause officers to engage verbally (and more politely) rather than resorting to “combat tactics” when, in fact, these tactics are needed. Placing ourselves in the officers’ shoes for a moment, it is unsurprising that BWCs could be linked to an increased rate of assaults. As Stetser (2001: 1) notes, officers were more likely to be assaulted in certain circumstances than others—at night, when suspects were intoxicated or had mental health problems, if the officer was new to the job, and so on.

    “[O]fficers were assaulted more frequently late at night and on weekends…unemployed and homeless suspects were more likely to assault…suspects who assaulted officers had usually been drinking, were under the influence of drugs, or had a mental disorder; newer officers were more likely to be injured in an assault than more experienced officers; older officers were more likely to be injured in an assault than younger officers…chemical sprays were usually effective in controlling suspects.”

    Thus, responding with force is a complex and heterogeneous phenomenon. When officers with BWCs are required to subdue certain perpetrators, the cameras may cause officers to behave too softly or can convey the message that the officer would not apply force as needed. Criminals may perceive this message as a “green light” to assault officers during stand-offs without the risk of retaliation by the officer.

  2. (2)

    “Skipping” stages on the force continuum. Our second interpretation of the increased ratio of assaults of officers is that the removal of discretion about turning on BWCs led officers to become over-cautious in using “aggressive voice commands.” Aggressive voice commands can be effective nonphysical restraining tactics. Officers can use firm commands (i.e., request/order/threat; see Fyfe 1988; Toch 1996; Worden 1995; on the “positioning” of voice commands within the force continuum more broadly, see Table 1 in Garner et al. 1995), which often take the form of shouting, cursing, and threats. These are essential tools in the officer’s toolbox against some suspects (for instance, those characterized as risk-taking, violent, and competitive—or the young male syndrome; see Wilson and Daly 1985). Nonphysical voice commands are, in fact, part and parcel of force and may be required in certain circumstances. In fact, for some forces, a “voice command” is third in a gradient continuum of seven stages, after “no force” and “police presence” (see Garner et al. 1995:158). After all, if order can be restored by using foul language rather than the use of a physical response, the former should be preferred; however, this demeanor from an officer does not look good on camera (for further on the use of profanity by officers, see Patton et al. 2017).

    As argued above, BWCs cause officers to behave in socially desirable ways. Therefore, if officers are reluctant to use these aggressive voice commands because cursing is deemed inappropriate by commanding officers, BWCs may cause officers to avoid using these tactics. As a result, in situations in which some force is needed, BWCs caused officers to skip stages in the force continuum and resort to physical reactions. Thus, officers may be reluctant to use nonphysical force, when such responses are needed, given the circumstances of the interaction. Instead, the situation escalates directly from no-force response into a physical response, without the necessary aggressive voice command. This skipping leads to a violent encounter, with officer injuries. In other words, because the officers feared a response by their supervisors for applying potentially unnecessary or disproportional nonphysical force or foul language, they were induced to apply physical force and consequently placed themselves in a position in which they could be assaulted.

In summary, although psychologists have already identified negative consequences of being observed in the psychopathological sense, we show that adverse consequences can occur in the context of formal control mechanisms as well. Undesirable behavioral manifestations found in previous research on self-awareness include self-harm, avoidance, and increased emotional intensity (Baumeister 1990; Morin and Craig 2000; Moskalenko and Heine 2003; Silvia 2002; Wain 2017). However, our findings suggest that under some circumstances, self-awareness can lead to excessive self-inspection that strips power-holders of their ability to function under extreme situations. We now know that use of force occurs relatively early in most interactions (Willits and Makin 2017), so split-second decision-making—or what Kahneman refers to as System I Thinking (Kahneman 2011)—such as the decision to apply force in the line of duty may suffer, when an officer is aware that his actions are observed and potentially scrutinized.

Unearthing the paradoxical effects

How can we explain the overall reduction in assaults with the simultaneous increase in the odds of assaults in the treatment conditions? The most parsimonious explanation seems to be as follows: overall, officers have changed the way they behave in public. BWCs have caused an organizational learning process that Ariel et al. (2016c) referred to as “contagious accountability”; regardless of whether BWCs were actually worn, police modified their decisions through self-awareness, which translated into different behavior. At the same time, some officers may have inhibited their behavior to such an extent that they did not apply the force necessary to spare them from being injured (what we call being over-deterred). We suspect that better training and experience can potentially counter these effects, and may eventually supplant the need for BWCs in the first place as officers engage better with citizens. However, it seems that BWCs are here to stay—so it is important to carry on studying their effects, intended or otherwise, to ensure they are deployed appropriately.

Hence, while we see an overall reduction in assaults on a year-to-year basis, in most of the participating police departments the treatment conditions experienced an increase in assaults compared to control conditions, while in other departments the reversed result emerged (three departments experienced the most prominent treatment effect; however, the meta-analytic approach, coupled with the statistical sensitivity analyses, indicates that the overall effect is consistent with an increase in assaults). The practical mechanism that enables this pattern is the activation policy embraced by the police department: the more restrictive and tighter the control over officers’ decisions, the more likely they will become self-aware and, by implication, less likely to react to aggressive encounters with the appropriate measures. The outcome of this self-awareness, channeled by the activation policy, is more assaults against the officers. Therefore, whether an officer is recording all of her or his contacts during the shift or only certain interactions with the public, could have profound differences on whether and how the officer changes behavior. Response to being recorded for all interactions could be vastly different than a response when only being recorded during an encounter the officer elects to record. This implies that departments should assist frontline officers in overcoming issues associated with the perceived politeness as weakness as well as the skipping through the stages of the force continuum, as we alluded to earlier, when using BWCs.

A final word about “assaults” against police officers

Different jurisdictions around the globe define “assaults against the police” in different ways. One prominent distinction is between verbal assaults and physical assaults. Verbal abuse can be considered a crime, depending on how vitriolic it is, to what extent it interferes with a police officer in her or his duties, and the circumstances of the case. Physical violence often falls under battery charges, hence the charge of assault and battery in many jurisdictions. However, in many jurisdictions across the United States, “contempt of a cop”, that is, a nonthreatening, nonphysical assault and disrespect of a police officer, is usually not deemed a criminal offense, as such discourse is often protected under a constitutional guard of freedom of speech.Footnote 6 On the other hand, in places like the United Kingdom and other countries, verbal transgressions may be considered notifiable offenses, when they cause “harassment, alarm or distress” of the officer.Footnote 7

In our experiments, we included both verbal as well as physical assaults of officers. We had no way to separate the type of assault, as we were limited by the provisions of our data-sharing agreements with the participating police departments. This caveat must be recognized, as we consider the practical implications of the study. Perhaps, for example, the increase in treatment group compared to control group is the result of an increase in primarily verbal incidents rather than physical assault (i.e., upon being notified that they are on camera, citizens try to taunt officers into misconduct by verbally assaulting them, knowing that officers cannot do much about it). On the other hand, we suspect that at least some of the assaults against of the officers were physical; however, we cannot determine how many. Consequently, it can be the case that officers wearing BWCs were able to de-escalate situations where often verbal abuse is common, but such cooling down while using BWCS is not feasible for situations involving physical abuse of officers. We cannot tell.

As our data do not allow us to make such inferences, we call instead for other scholars to pay a closer attention to the distinction between types of assaults than we could, when designing trials on BWCs. At the same time, we note that both verbal and nonverbal aggression at the workplace are considered as a serious concern. Workplace violence has been found to correlate significantly with psychological strain, burn-out fatigue, perceptions of danger, anxiety, and potentially PTSD (see Alkus and Padesky 1983; Anshel 2000; Burke and Deszca 1986; MacDonald et al. 2003; Robinson et al. 1997). Even if the effect concentrates on nonverbal aggression, a backfiring effect of BWCs on police officers’ wellbeing can lead to both personal as well as organizational adverse consequences, a perverse consequence we ought to minimize.