Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Exploring the effects of concreteness fading across grades in elementary school science education

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigates the effects that concreteness fading has on learning and transfer across three grade levels (4–6) in elementary school science education in comparison to learning with constantly concrete representations. 127 9- to 12-years-old elementary school students studied electric circuits in a computer-based simulation environment, where circuits remained concrete (bulbs) throughout the learning or faded from concrete to abstract (bulbs to resistors). The most important finding was that the outcomes seemed to be influenced by a developmental factor: the study found a significant interaction between condition and grade level in relation to learning outcomes, suggesting that the outcomes generally improved as a function of grade level, but that there were notable differences between the conditions regarding the improvement of outcomes across the three grades. According the results, learning with constantly concrete representations either took less time or resulted in better learning compared to concreteness fading. Because transfer is one of the central arguments for concreteness fading, a somewhat surprising finding was that the concrete condition succeeded at least as well as the fading condition on transfer tasks. The study also discusses why the results and issues related to the conceptualisation and operationalisation of central concepts in the study call for caution towards generalization and for more research with young learners across different grades.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The choice for resistors as abstract representations is not evident from a formal physics point of view, whereof a resistor and a bulb are equivalent and equally concrete circuit components. The physics view, however, presupposes notable formal knowledge about the domain. Given the fact that the students had no previous formal education on electricity it is highly unlikely that they already shared such conception. This contention is backed up by results of several studies that show that students, even on a high school level, perceive bulbs and resistors differently (Jaakkola and Veermans 2015; Johnson et al. 2013; Moreno et al. 2011). An explanations for this difference, and an argument for the assertion that bulbs are more concrete than resistors, is that from a conceptual or perceptual point of view, bulbs have a clear practical purpose (to radiate light) that based on prior everyday experiences practically everyone can relate to. In contrast, the purpose of resistors to reduce current flow and divide voltages in a circuit, among many other uses, is unclear and significantly more abstract to most people.

  2. The initial sample consisted of 134 students but only those were included in the analyses who completed all three parts of the study (pre-test, intervention, and post-test).

  3. The students worked in pairs because working in pairs is a natural procedure in science classrooms in Finland, which at the same time ensured better comparability with the initial study that also included pairs (Jaakkola and Veermans 2015).

  4. g is Hedges' (1981) bias corrected standardised mean difference effect size for two independent samples.

References

  • Ainsworth, S. (1999). The functions of multiple representations. Computers & Education, 33(23), 131–152. doi:10.1016/S0360-1315(99)00029-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, S. (2006). DeFT: A conceptual framework for considering learning with multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 16(3), 183–198. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Situated learning and education. Educational Researcher, 25(4), 5–11. doi:10.3102/0013189X025004005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite, D. W., & Goldstone, R. L. (2013). Integrating formal and grounded representations in combinatorics learning. Journal of Educational Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0032095.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., Zaitchik, D., & Bascandziev, I. (2015). Theories of development: In dialog with Jean Piaget. Developmental Review, 38, 36–54. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5(2), 121–152. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, B. D. (1997). The rediscovery of the active learner in adaptive contexts: A developmental-historical analysis. Educational Psychologist, 32(1), 41–55. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3201_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Bock, D., Deprez, J., Van Dooren, W., Roelens, M., & Verschaffel, L. (2011). Abstract or concrete examples in learning mathematics? A replication and elaboration of Kaminski, Sloutsky, and Heckler’s study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 42(2), 109–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., & Mouyi, A. (2011). Educating the developing mind: Towards an overarching paradigm. Educational Psychology Review, 23(4), 601–663. doi:10.1007/s10648-011-9178-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, N. D., Adams, W. K., Keller, C. J., Kohl, P. B., Perkins, K. K., Podolefsky, N. S., … LeMaster, R. (2005). When learning about the real world is better done virtually: A study of substituting computer simulations for laboratory equipment. Physical Review Special TopicsPhysics Education Research, 1(1), 010103. doi:10.1103/PhysRevSTPER.1.010103

  • Fyfe, E. R., McNeil, N. M., & Borjas, S. (2015). Benefits of “concreteness fading” for children’s mathematics understanding. Learning and Instruction, 35, 104–120. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.10.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fyfe, E. R., McNeil, N. M., Son, J. Y., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Concreteness fading in mathematics and science instruction: A systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 26(1), 9–25. doi:10.1007/s10648-014-9249-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 393–405. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Sakamoto, Y. (2003). The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 414–466. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00519-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(1), 69–110. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1401_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6(2), 107–128. doi:10.2307/1164588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaakkola, T., & Nurmi, S. (2008). Fostering elementary school students’ understanding of simple electricity by combining simulation and laboratory activities. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 24(4), 271–283. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00259.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaakkola, T., Nurmi, S., & Lehtinen, E. (2010). Conceptual change in learning electricity: Using virtual and concrete external representations simultaneously. In E. Verschaffel, E. De Corte, T. de Jong, & J. Elen (Eds.), Use of representations in reasoning and problem solving. Analysis and improvement (pp. 133–152). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaakkola, T., Nurmi, S., & Veermans, K. (2011). A comparison of students’ conceptual understanding of electric circuits in simulation only and simulation-laboratory contexts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(1), 71–93. doi:10.1002/tea.20386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaakkola, T., & Veermans, K. (2015). Effects of abstract and concrete simulation elements on science learning. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 31(4), 300–313. doi:10.1111/jcal.12089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, A. M., Butcher, K. R., Ozogul, G., & Reisslein, M. (2013). Learning from abstract and contextualized representations: The effect of verbal guidance. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 2239–2247. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J. A., Sloutsky, V. M., & Heckler, A. F. (2008). The advantage of abstract examples in learning math. Science, 320(5875), 454–455. doi:10.1126/science.1154659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurtz, K. J., Miao, C.-H., & Gentner, D. (2001). Learning by analogical bootstrapping. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10(4), 417–446. doi:10.2307/1466739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11(1), 65–100. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6708.1987.tb00863.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 31–48). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, L. C., & Shaffer, P. S. (1992). Research as a guide for curriculum development: An example from introductory electricity. Part I: Investigation of student understanding. American Journal of Physics, 60(11), 994. doi:10.1119/1.17003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, N. M., & Fyfe, E. R. (2012). “Concreteness fading” promotes transfer of mathematical knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 22(6), 440–448. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., Ozogul, G., & Reisslein, M. (2011). Teaching with concrete and abstract visual representations: Effects on students’ problem solving, problem representations, and learning perceptions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 32–47. doi:10.1037/a0021995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pape, S. J., & Tchoshanov, M. A. (2001). The role of representation(s) in developing mathematical understanding. Theory into Practice, 40(2), 118–127. doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4002_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naive physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions. Cognition and Instruction, 18(1), 1–34. doi:10.2307/3233798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanism of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24(2), 113–142. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2402_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W. (2005). An integrated model of text and picture comprehension. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 49–60). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Siler, S. A., & Klahr, D. (2016). Effects of terminological concreteness on middle-school students’ learning of experimental design. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 547–562. doi:10.1037/edu0000072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning versus performance: An integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 176–199. doi:10.1177/1745691615569000

  • Tabachneck-Schijf, H. J., Leonardo, A. M., & Simon, H. A. (1997). CaMeRa: A computational model of multiple representations. Cognitive Science, 21(3), 305–350. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2103_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Targiso Borges, A. (1999). Mental models of electricity. International Journal of Science Education, 21(1), 95–117. doi:10.1080/095006999290859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. T., & Gunstone, R. F. (1992). Probing understanding. London: Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by research grant (no 266189) from the Academy of Finland to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tomi Jaakkola.

Appendix

Appendix

Fig. 5
figure 5

A translated and compacted example of a worksheet in the concrete condition (CC)

Fig. 6
figure 6

A translated and compacted example of a worksheet in the fading condition (FC)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jaakkola, T., Veermans, K. Exploring the effects of concreteness fading across grades in elementary school science education. Instr Sci 46, 185–207 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-017-9428-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-017-9428-y

Keywords

Navigation