Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of different levels of disfluency on learning outcomes and cognitive load

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In some cases difficult-to-read learning materials lead to better performance than easy to-read materials. This phenomenon is called the disfluency effect and is attributed to a subjective feeling of task difficulty resulting in a deeper learning approach. In two studies (n1 = 67, n2 = 73) we investigated the impact of three increasing disfluency levels compared to a fluent condition on learning outcomes, in the second study we additionally addressed effects on cognitive load and time on task. The first study revealed significant differences of the experimental groups and on a descriptive level a reversed u-shape pattern, i.e., learning performance increases with increasing disfluency but decreases when the text becomes too illegible. The same pattern could be found significantly in the second study for recall and transfer performance, engagement and time on task and the reversed pattern for extraneous load. Results indicate that there is in fact an optimal level of disfluency on performance that comes along with increased engagement and that there is a breaking point of disfluency where the extraneous load seems to be too high.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 219–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 136, 569–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bannert, M. (2002). Managing cognitive load—Recent trends in cognitive load theory. Learning and Instruction, 12, 139–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Wänke, M., Bohner, G., Fellhauer, R. F., & Schwarz, N. (1994). Need for cognition: eine Skala zur Erfassung von Engagement und Freude bei Denkaufgaben. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 25, 147–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2007). It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech effect language comprehension. Cognition, 105, 658–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 104, 268–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the bold (and the italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118, 114–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eitel, A., & Kühl, T. (2016). Effects of disfluency and test expectancy on learning with text. Metacognition and Learning, 11, 107–121. doi:10.1007/s11409-015-9145-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eitel, A., Kühl, T., Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (2014). Disfluency meets cognitive load in multimedia learning: Does harder-to-read mean better-to-understand? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 488–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., & Harmann, H. H. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • French, M. M. J., Blood, A., Bright, N. D., Futak, D., Grohmann, M. J., Hasthorpe, A., et al. (2013). Changing fonts in education: How the benefits vary with ability and dyslexia. The Journal of Educational Research, 106, 301–304. doi:10.1080/00220671.2012.736430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, R. M., & Kintsch, W. (1971). Differential effects of study and test trials on long-term recognition and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 562–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1890).

  • Kalyuga, S. (2005). Prior knowledge principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 325–338). Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., & Singh, A. (2015). Rethinking the boundaries of cognitive load theory in complex learning. Educational Psychology Review. doi:10.1007/s10648-015-9352-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 379–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klepsch, M., & Seufert, T. (2012). Subjective differentiated measurement of cognitive load. Paper presented at 5th international cognitive load theory conference, Tallahassee, USA.

  • Kühl, T., & Eitel, A. (2016). Effects of disfluency on cognitive and metacognitive processes and outcomes. Metacognition and Learning, 11, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kühl, T., Eitel, A., Damnik, G., & Körndle, H. (2014). The impact of disfluency, pacing, and students’ need for cognition on learning with multimedia. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 189–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, J., Goussios, C., & Seufert, T. (2016). Working memory capacity and disfluency effect: An aptitude-treatment-interaction study. Metacognition and Learning, 11, 89–105. doi:10.1007/s11409-015-9149-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leppink, J., Paas, F., van der Vleuten, C., van Gog, T., & van Merrienboer, J. (2013). Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behavior Research Methods, 4, 1058–1072. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0334-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2014). Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed.). Cambride: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mengelkamp, C., Pieger, E., & Bannert, M. (2015). Using eye-tracking to investigate the effects of disfluency on metacomprehension. Paper presented at the 16th EARLI Conference for research on learning and instruction: synergies between learning, teaching and research, Limassol, Cyprus.

  • Moreno, R. (2005). Instructional technology: Promise and pitfalls. In L. PytlikZillig, M. Bodvarsson, & R. Bruning (Eds.), Technology-based education: Bringing researchers and practitioners together (pp. 1–19). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., Süß, H.-M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W. (2000). Working memory capacity—facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1017–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M., & Alter, A. L. (2014). The search for moderators in disfluency research. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 502–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 338–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2001). QCM: A questionnaire to assess current motivation in learning situations (original title: FAM: Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung aktueller Motivation in Lern-und Leistungssituationen). Diagnostica, 47, 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 137, 615–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (1999). Einflüsse der Visualisierungsform auf die Konstruktion mentaler Modelle beim Text- und Bildverstehen. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 46, 217–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 475–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seufert, T. (2003). Supporting coherence formation in learning from multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 13, 227–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Fluency and the detection of misleading questions: Low processing fluency attenuates the Moses illusion. Social Cognition, 26, 791–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sungkhasettee, V. W., Friedman, M. C., & Castel, A. D. (2011). Memory and metamemory for inverted words: Illusions of competency and desirable difficulties. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 973–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tverski, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yue, C. L., Castel, A. D., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When disfluency is—and is not—a desirable difficulty: The influence of typeface clarity on metacognitive judgments and memory. Memory and Cognition, 41, 229–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We especially thank Wolfgang Schnotz and Maria Bannert for providing us the learning material of the second study in addition to all the conceptual thoughts we could learn from them. We also thank Nina Singer and Verena Fritz for their support in conducting the study. Moreover, we appreciate the very thoughtful comments of two anonymous reviewers who helped us to improve the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tina Seufert.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Seufert, T., Wagner, F. & Westphal, J. The effects of different levels of disfluency on learning outcomes and cognitive load. Instr Sci 45, 221–238 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-016-9387-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-016-9387-8

Keywords

Navigation