Skip to main content
Log in

Individual versus collaborative problem solving: divergent outcomes depending on task complexity

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many studies have tested external supports for promoting productive collaboration, but relatively few have examined what features characterize naturally productive collaborative tasks. Two lines of research have come to distinct conclusions on the primary task feature associated with productive collaboration: demonstrability versus complexity. This study examined the problem-solving performance of 110 seventh grade students on a demonstrable mathematical task, including 69 in three traditional math classrooms (for whom the task was complex) and 41 in two accelerated math classrooms (for whom the task was not complex). Students were further assigned to one of four conditions split by two factors: grouping (individual versus dyad) and number of problems (one or two). For the accelerated math classes, individuals performed significantly better than dyads. For the traditional math classes, dyads performed significantly better than individuals and exceeded the truth-wins criterion (a theoretical maximum indicating how individuals would perform if they shared knowledge perfectly). A complex-demonstrable task framework is proposed for characterizing naturally productive collaborative tasks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The calculation for the expected group performance is based on the work of Lorge and Solomon (1955). Since we know the proportion of individuals that could solve the husband and wives problem (14 %), we can calculate the probability that groups from this population will have at least one person that can find the correct answer to the problem. The easiest way to do this is to find the probability that the group contains no one that can solve the problem and subtract from one (e.g., if there is a 10 % chance no one in a group can solve the problem, then there is a 90 % chance that at least one person can). If Q is the proportion of individuals who cannot solve the problem, the probability that a group of size n contains no one that can solve the problem is Qn. In the husband and wives problem the group size is 4 and the proportion of individuals that cannot solve the problem is Q = (1−0.14) = 0.86. Thus, the probability that the four-person group contains at least one individuals that can solve the problem is 1−0.864 ~45%.

  2. The degrees of freedom are adjusted due to heterogeneity of variance between conditions. The unadjusted results are similar, t(108) = 3.238, p = .002.

  3. Results of all analyses were the same with or without the Pretest score entered as a covariate.

  4. This does not include four groups of three traditional math students (all of whom also found at least one solution to the problem). The proportion would have been even greater, 19 out of 21, had the triads been included.

References

  • Antil, L. R., Jenkins, J. R., Wayne, S. K., & Vadasy, P. F. (1998). Co-operative learning research: Prevalence, conceptualizations and the relation between research and practice. American Educational Research Journal, 35, 419–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. S., & Kerr, N. L. (2003). Group process, group decision, group action (p. 41). Buckingham: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, B. (2000). Problem solving in video-based microworlds: Collaborative and individual outcomes of high-achieving sixth-grade students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 391–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, B. (2003). When smart groups fail. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12, 307–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, N. T., Stephan, C., Rosenfield, D., Aronson, E., & Sikes, J. (1977). Interdependence in the classroom: A field study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C. K. K. (2001). Peer collaboration and discourse patterns in learning from incompatible information. Instructional Science, 29, 443–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. G. (1994). Restructuring the classroom: Conditions for productive small groups. Review of Educational Research, 64, 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. G., & Lotan, R. A. (Eds.). (1997). Working for equity in heterogeneous classrooms: Sociological theory in practice. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. G., Lotan, R. A., & Leechor, C. (1989). Can classrooms learn? Sociology of Education, 62, 75–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. G., Lotan, R. A., Scarloss, B. A., & Arellano, A. R. (1999). Complex instruction: Equity in cooperative learning classrooms. Theory into Practice, 38, 81–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E., Lotan, R., Scarloss, B., Schultz, S., & Abram, P. (2002). Can groups learn? Teachers College Record, 104, 1045–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, D. J., & Kagel, J. H. (2005). Are two heads better than one? Team versus individual play in signaling games. American Economic Review, 95(3), 477–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, A. R., & Valacich, J. S. (1993). Computer brainstorms: More heads are better than one. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(4), 531–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Towards the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 497–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, B. F., Immekus, J. C., & Oakes, W. C. (2005). An examination of indicators of engineering students’ success and persistence. Journal of Engineering Education, 94, 419–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M., & Holyoak, K. (1983). Scheme Induction and Analogical Transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, G. W. (1982). Group versus individual performance: Are N + 1 heads better than one? Psychological Bulletin, 91(3), 517–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, J., Erkens, G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kanselaar, G. (2010). Effects of representational guidance during computer-supported collaborative learning. Instructional Science, 38, 59–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research. Edina: Interaction Book Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational psychology success story: Social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educational Researcher, 38(5), 365–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2010). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science, 38, 523–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. L., & Bruun, S. E. (1983). Dispensability of member effort and group motivation losses: Free-rider effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 78–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review in Psychology, 55, 623–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, U.-M., & Stark, R. (2010). Reflection in example- and problem-based learning: Effects of reflection prompts, feedback and cooperative learning. Evaluation & Research in Education, 23, 255–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 823–832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., & Adamopoulos, J. A. (1980). Social combination processes and individual learning for six-person cooperative groups on an intellective task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 941–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., & Barth, J. M. (1981). Group-to-individual and individual-to-group problem-solving transfer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 1087–1093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., Bonner, B. L., & Miner, A. G. (2002). Group performs better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers problems. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 605–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., Carey, H. R., & Kerr, N. L. (2008). Group-to-individual problem-solving transfer. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11(3), 319–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., & Ellis, A. L. (1986). Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 177–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., Hatch, E. C., Silver, J. S., & Boh, L. (2006). Groups perform better than the best individual on letters-to-numbers problems: Effects of group size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 644–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., Zander, M. L., Knievel, E. M., & Tan, T. K. (2003). Groups perform better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers problems: Informative equations and effective reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 684–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorge, I., & Solomon, H. (1955). Two models of group behavior in the solution of eureka type problems. Psychometrika, 20(2), 139–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marton, F. (2006). Sameness and difference in transfer. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15, 499–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, G. M., Wolfe, H., Besterfield-Sacre, M., & Shuman, L. J. (2010). Predicting STEM degree outcomes based on eighth grade data and standard test scores. Journal of Engineering Education, 99, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, A. M. (1996). Effects of explicit incentives on scripted and unscripted cooperation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 74–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 781–802). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olivera, F., & Straus, S. G. (2004). Group-to-individual transfer of learning. Small Group Research, 35, 440–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saleh, M., Lazonder, A. W., & de Jong, T. (2005). Effects of within-class ability grouping on social interaction, achievement, and motivation. Instructional Science, 33, 105–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L. (1995). The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(3), 321–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 475–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, D. A., & Pai, H.-H. (2012). Effects of cooperative versus individual study on learning and motivation after reward-removal. Journal of Experimental Education, 80, 246–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, M. E. (1932). A comparison of individuals and small groups in the rational solution of complex problems. The American Journal of Psychology, 44(3), 491–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E. (1995). Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice (2nd ed.). Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E. (1996). Research for the future: Research on cooperative learning and achievement: What we know, what we need to know. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 43–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stasson, M. F., Kameda, T., Parks, C. D., Zimmerman, S. K., & Davis, J. H. (1991). Effects of assigned group consensus requirements on group problem solving and group members’ learning. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stegmann, K., Wecker, C., Weinberger, A., & Fischer, F. (2011). Collaborative argumentation and cognitive elaboration in a computer-supported collaborative learning environment. Instructional Science,. doi:10.1007/s11251-011-9174-5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steiner, I. D. (1966). Models for inferring relationships between group size and potential group productivity. Behavioral Science, 11(4), 273–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiner, I. D. (1972). Group process and productivity. New York: Academic press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuovinen, J. E., & Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load associated with discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 334–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, N. M., Franke, M. L., Ing, M., Chan, A., De, T., Freund, D., et al. (2008). The role of teacher instructional practices in student collaboration. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 360–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, A., Ertl, B., Fischer, F., & Mandl, H. (2005). Epistemic and social scripts in computer-supported collaborative learning. Instructional Science, 33, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitin, P., & Whitin, D. (2004). The Magic Triangle Puzzle. Retrieved Jan 21, 2013, from http://www.readwritethink.org/lesson_images/lesson820/TrianglePuzzle.pdf.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by a Kinley Trust research grant to David Sears. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this work are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Kinley Trust. Both authors contributed equally to this manuscript. They would like to thank the principal, teacher, and students for making this research possible. Additional thanks go to Jammie Chang, James Greenan, and anonymous reviewers for their feedback on this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. Sears.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sears, D.A., Reagin, J.M. Individual versus collaborative problem solving: divergent outcomes depending on task complexity. Instr Sci 41, 1153–1172 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-013-9271-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-013-9271-8

Keywords

Navigation