Skip to main content
Log in

Learning about bones at a science museum: examining the alternate hypotheses of ceiling effect and prior knowledge

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Groups of children at a science museum were pre- and post-assessed with a type of concept map, known as personal meaning maps, to determine what new understandings, if any, they were gaining from participation in a series of structured hands-on activities about bones and the process of bones healing. Close examination was made regarding whether children’s prior knowledge or a ceiling effect was influencing results. Children made significant gains in vocabulary and concepts related to both bones and the bone healing process. Many children also demonstrated that their comprehension moved from a novice level to a transitional level of understanding. Prior to participation, children were more uniformly unacquainted with ideas about the healing process of bones than they were about bones; this led to more consistent learning gains related to the healing process. There was some indication of a ceiling effect occurring when children revealed what they had learned about bones, but not when they revealed what they had learned about the bone healing process. Although the prior knowledge theory was not statistically supported, data did show that children with greater understanding prior to the Busy Bones Lab activities end up with correspondingly greater understanding. This suggests that addressing concepts related to bones prior to a lab experience may bring more children to a higher level of understanding before engaging in the lab experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander, P. A., Kulikowich, J. M., & Jetton, T. L. (1994). The role of subject-matter knowledge and interest in the processing of linear and nonlinear texts. Review of Educational Research, 64(2), 201–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, M. P. (2006). Visual representations in science education: The influence of prior knowledge and cognitive load theory on instructional design principles. Science Education, 90(6), 1073–1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dochy, F. J. R. C., & Alexander, P. A. (1995). Mapping prior knowledge: A framework for discussion among researchers. European Journal for Psychology of Education, 10(1), 123–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dochy, F., Segers, M., & Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between assessment practices and outcomes of studies: The case of research on prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research, 69(2), 145–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, M. S., & Bransford, J. D. (Eds.). (2005). How students learn: History, mathematics, and Science in the classroom. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, K. M., Luke, J. J., & Dierking, L. D. (2004). Family learning research in museums: An emerging disciplinary matrix? Science Education, 88(S1), S48–S58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. H., Moussouri, T., & Coulson, D. (1998). The effect of visitors’ agendas on museum learning. Curator, 41(2), 106–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J., & Storksdieck, M. (2005). Using the contextual model of learning to understand visitor learning from a science center exhibition. Science Education, 89(5), 744–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M. F., Cooke, C. L., & Laberge, M. J. (1983). Effects of previewing difficult short stories on low ability junior high school students’ comprehension, recall, and attitudes. Reading Research Quarterly, 18(3), 262–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewson, M. G., & Hewson, P. W. (1983). Effect of instruction using students’ prior knowledge and conceptual change strategies on science learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(8), 731–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, J. H., & Dwyer, F. M. (1984). The effects of prior knowledge, presentation mode, and visual realism on student achievement. Journal of Experimental Education, 52(2), 110–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lelliott, A. (2007). Learning about Astronomy: A case study exploring how grade 7 and 8 students experience sites of informal learning in South Africa. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa.

  • Linton, T. H., & Kester, D. (2003). Exploring the achievement gap between white and minority students in Texas: A comparison of the 1996 and 2000 NAEP and TAAS eighth grade mathematics test results. Education Policy Analysis Archives, 11(10). Retrieved January 21, 2011, from http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v11n10/.

  • Liu, S., & Lederman, N. G. (2002). Taiwanese gifted students’ views of nature of science. School Science and Mathematics, 102(3), 114–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luke, J., O’Mara, H., & Dierking, L. (1999). Mammals hall front-end evaluation phase II, National Museum of Natural History. Unpublished evaluation report. Annapolis, MD: Institute for Learning Innovation.

  • McBee, M. (2010). Modeling outcomes with floor or ceiling effects: An introduction to the Tobit Model. Gifted Child Quarterly, 54(4), 314–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Sinatra, G. M., & Loxterman, J. A. (1992). The relative contribution of prior knowledge and coherent text to comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 78–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozuru, Y., Dempsey, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2009). Prior knowledge, reading skill, and text cohesion in the comprehension of science texts. Learning and Instruction, 19(3), 228–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prokop, P., Fancovicová, J., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2009). The effect of type of instruction on expression of children’s knowledge: How do children see the endocrine and urinary system? International Journal of Environmental & Science Education, 4(1), 75–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, M., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2001). Students’ understanding of their internal structure as revealed by drawings. In H. Behrendt, H. Dahncke, R. Duit, W. Graber, M. Komorek, A. Kross, & P. Reiska (Eds.), Research in science education—Past, present, and future (pp. 101–106). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rennie, L. J., Feher, E., Dierking, L. D., & Falk, J. H. (2003). Toward an agenda for advancing research on science learning in out-of-school settings. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(2), 112–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rifkin, B. (2005). A ceiling effect in traditional classroom foreign language instruction: Data from Russian. The Modern Language Journal, 89(1), 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rivet, A. E., & Krajcik, J. S. (2008). Contextualizing instruction: Leveraging students’ prior knowledge and experiences to foster understanding of middle school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45(1), 79–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J. (1995). Learning in interactive environments: Prior knowledge and new experience. In L. D. Dierking & J. Falk (Eds.), Public institutions for personal learning: Establishing a research agenda (pp. 37–51). Washington, DC: American Association of Museums.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strangman, N., & Hall, T. (2004). Background knowledge. Wakefield, MA: National Center on Accessing the General Curriculum. Retrieved January 25, 2011, from http://www.aim.cast.org/learn/historyarchive/backgroundpapers/background_knowledge.

  • Uttl, B. (2005). Measurements of individual differences: Lessons from memory assessment in research and clinical practice. Psychological Science, 16(6), 460–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Zhang, Z., McArdle, J. J., & Salthouse, T. A. (2008). Investigating ceiling effects in longitudinal data analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 43(3), 476–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Framing New Pathways to Medical Discovery for Families, Students and Teachers grant (PI: Dr. Laura Martin, Award Number R25RR026032) at the Arizona Science Center, funded by the Science Education Partnership Award from the National Center for Research Resources, a division of the National Institutes of Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Center For Research Resources or the National Institutes of Health. The author gratefully acknowledges the assistance of Dr. Laura Martin and Mr. Deron Ash for coordinating the research opportunity.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eugene Judson.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Busy Bones Lab activity flowchart

figure a

Appendix 2

 

(1) Novice understanding

(2) Transitional understanding

(3) Informed understanding

Depth of Understanding Bones Rubric

 Bones

The personal meaning map reflects limited knowledge of bones. The map may be restricted to a few nouns associated with bones. Items mentioned in the map may be unconnected or unrelated to bones. The map may yield only one or two basic words/phrases

The personal meaning map reflects age suitable rudimentary understanding of bones. Transitional understanding is often distinguished from novice understanding when the map indicates fair understanding of bones as being alive and/or critical to the body (e.g., for movement, protection). The map may reveal concept(s) related to bones without demonstrating deep understanding

The personal meaning map reflects age appropriate comprehension of bones. Informed understanding is often distinguished from transitional understanding when the map indicates developed understanding of bones as being alive and/or critical to the body and/or uses sophisticated terms correctly. The map need not provide complete expert knowledge, but does reflect a clear understanding of ideas recorded

Depth of Understanding Bones Healing Rubric

 Bones Healing

The personal meaning map reflects limited knowledge of how bones heal. The map may be restricted to a few nouns associated with bones or with peripheral features of bone healing (e.g., casts). Items mentioned in the map may be unconnected or unrelated to the concept of healing

The personal meaning map reflects age suitable rudimentary understanding of one or more elements of the process of bones healing. Transitional understanding is often distinguished from novice understanding when the map indicates comprehension that healing processes that are not visible on the surface are occurring. The map may reveal concept(s) of bone healing (e.g., time is required or marrow involved), but process is not well depicted

The personal meaning map reflects age appropriate comprehension of stages and/or processes associated with bones healing. The map need not provide complete expert knowledge, but does reflect a clear understanding of ideas recorded

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Judson, E. Learning about bones at a science museum: examining the alternate hypotheses of ceiling effect and prior knowledge. Instr Sci 40, 957–973 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9201-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9201-6

Keywords

Navigation