Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Asymmetrical Effects of Justice Sensitivity Perspectives on Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Three studies explore the effects of perspective-specific justice sensitivity on indicators of both prosocial behavior (i.e., existential guilt, solidarity, and responsibility ascriptions towards the disadvantaged) and antisocial behavior (i.e., the willingness to transgress a norm in a moral temptation dilemma). On the basis of theoretical considerations and earlier findings it is expected that being sensitive towards injustice from a beneficiary’s perspective is associated positively with prosocial and negatively with antisocial behavior, whereas the opposite should be true for being sensitive towards injustice from a victim’s perspective. The results from all three studies support these hypotheses. It is argued that JS-beneficiary indicates a genuine, “other-oriented” concern for justice and social responsibility, whereas JS-victim indicates a mixture of “self-related” and justice-related concerns.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, G. R. (1977). Physical attractiveness research. Toward a developmental social psychology of beauty. Hum. Dev. 20: 217–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allon, N. (1982). The stigma of overweight in everyday life. In Woldman, B. B. (ed.), Psychological Aspects of Obesity, Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, pp. 130–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R. (1984). The Evolution of Cooperation, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1990). Selective activation and disengagement of moral control. J. Soc. Issues 46: 27–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., and Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71: 364–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Kobrynowicz, D., Dinnerstein, J. L., Kampf, H. C., and Wilson, A. D. (1997). In a very different voice: Unmasking moral hypocrisy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72: 1335–1348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Thompson, E. R., Seuferling, G., Whitney, H., and Strongman, J. A. (1999). Moral hypocrisy: Appearing moral to oneself without being so. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77: 525–537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., and Daniels, L. R. (1964). Affecting the salience of the social responsibility norm. Effects of past help on the response to dependency relationships. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 68: 275–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bersoff, D. M. (1999). Why good people sometimes do bad things: Motivated reasoning and unethical behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25: 28–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierhoff, H. W., and Rohrmann, E. (2004). Altruistic personality in the context of the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Eur. J. Pers. 18: 351–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychol. Bull. 88: 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, J. M., and Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 11: 232–239.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson, P. (1996). The Bystander, Whurr, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 163–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dar, Y., and Resh, N. (2001). Exploring the multifaceted structure of sense of deprivation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 31: 63–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dar, Y., and Resh, N. (2003). Social disadvantage and students’ perceived injustice in socially integrated schools in Israel. Soc. Justice Res. 16: 109–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44: 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCremer, D., and van Lange, P. A. M. (2001). Why prosocials exhibit greater cooperation than proselfs: The roles of social responsibility and reciprocity. Eur. J. Pers. 15: 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K. L., and Dion, K. K. (1987). Belief in a just world and physical attractiveness stereotyping. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52: 775–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K. K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24: 207–213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fetchenhauer, D., and Huang, X. (2003). Justice sensitivity and behavior in experimental games. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 36: 1015–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S. A., and Bless, E. (1967). Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 7(2, PT.1): 117–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, M. (2004). Do normative transgressions affect punitive judgments? An empirical test of the psychoanalytic scapegoat hypothesis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30: 1650–1660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, M. R., and Hirschi, T. (1990). A General Theory of Crime, Stanford University Press, Stanford, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartshorne, H., and May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the Nature of Character, Vol. 1: Studies in Deceit, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Empathy, its limitations, and its role in a comprehensive moral theory. In Kurtines, W. M., and Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Morality, Moral Behavior, and Moral Development, Wiley, New York, pp. 283–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., and Miles, E. W. (1985). Test for individual perceptions of job equity: Some preliminary findings. Percept. Motor Skills 61: 1055–1064.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., and Miles, E. W. (1987). A new perspective on equity theory: The equity sensitivity construct. Acad. Manage. Rev. 12: 222–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). The Meaning and Measurement of Moral Development, Clark University Press, Worcester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konecni, V. J. (1972). Some effects of guilt on compliance: A field replication. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 23: 30–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konoske, P., Staple, S., and Graf, R. G. (1979). Compliant reactions to guilt: Self-esteem or self-punishment. J. Soc. Psychol. 108: 207–211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 108: 480–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landy, D., and Sigall, H. (1974). Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a function of the performer’s physical attractiveness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 29: 299–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovas, L. (1995). Nespravodlivost v interpersonalnych vztahoch [Injustice in interpersonal relations]. Ceskoslov. Psychol. 34: 203–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovas, L., and Pirhacova, I. (1996). Anxieta, hnevlivost a senzitivita voci nespravodlivosti [Anxiety, anger, and sensitivity to injustice]. Ceskoslov. Psychol. 40: 248–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovas, L., and Wolt, R. (2002). Sensitivity to injustice in the context of some personality traits. Stud. Psychol. 44: 125–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maes, J., and Schmitt, M. (1999). More on ultimate and immament justice: Results from the research project “Justice as a Problem within Reunified Germany.” Soc. Justice Res. 12: 65–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikula, G. (1994). Perspective-related differences in interpretations of injustice by victims and victimizers: A test with close relationships. In Lerner, M. J., and Mikula, G. (eds.), Entitlement and the Affectional Bond, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 175–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohiyeddini, C. (1998). Sensibilität für widerfahrene Ungerechtigkeit als Persönlichkeitseigenschaft [Sensitivity towards befallen injustice as a trait]. In Reichle, B., and Schmitt, M. (eds.), Verantwortung, Gerechtigkeit und Moral, Juventa, Weinheim, pp. 201–212.

  • Mohiyeddini, C., and Schmitt, M. (1997). Sensitivity to befallen injustice and reactions to unfair treatment in a laboratory situation. Soc. Justice Res. 10: 333–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montada, L., Schmitt, M., and Dalbert, C. (1986). Thinking about justice and dealing with one’s own privileges: A study of existential guilt. In Bierhoff, H. W., Cohen, R. L., and Greenberg, J. (eds.), Justice in Social Relations, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 125–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piliavin, J. A., and Piliavin, I. M. (1972). The effect of blood on reactions to a victim. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 23: 253–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piliavin, J. A., Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., and Clark, R. D. III (1981). Emergency Intervention, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riddle, M., and Roberts, A. H. (1977). Delinquency, delay of gratification, recidivism, and the Porteus Maze Tests. Psychol. Bull. 84: 417–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Z., and Peplau, L. A. (1973). Belief in a just world and reactions to another’s lot: A study of participants in the national draft lottery. J. Soc. Issues 29: 73–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen injustice. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 21: 3–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., and Dörfel, M. (1999). Procedural injustice at work, justice sensitivity, job satisfaction and psychosomatic well-being. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29: 443–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., and Maes, J. (1998). Perceived injustice in unified Germany and mental health. Soc. Justice Res. 11: 59–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., and Maes, J. (2002). Stereotypic ingroup bias as self-defense against relative deprivation: Evidence from a longitudinal study of the German unification process. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 32: 309–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., and Mohiyeddini, C. (1996). Sensitivity to befallen injustice and reactions to a real life disadvantage. Soc. Justice Res. 9: 223–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., Neumann, R., and Montada, L. (1995). Dispositional sensitivity to befallen injustice. Soc. Justice Res. 8: 385–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, M., Gollwitzer, M., Maes, J., and Arbach, D. (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess.

  • Schmitt, M., Behner, R., Montada, L., Müller, L., and Müller-Fohrbrodt, G. (2000). Gender, ethnicity, and education as privileges: Exploring the generalizability of the existential guilt reaction. Soc. Justice Res. 13: 313–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 10, Academic Press, New York, pp. 221–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H., and Howard, J. A. (1980). Explanations of the moderating effect of responsibility denial on the personal norm-behavior relationship. Soc. Psychol. Q. 43: 441–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skitka, L. J. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice reasoning. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28: 588–597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation. Sustaining the integrity of the self. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 21, Academic Press, New York, pp. 261–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sykes, G. M., and Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. Am. Sociol. Rev. 22: 664–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. B., Green, M. C., and Lerner, J. S. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78: 853–870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Maas, M., Waldring, I., and Semin, G. P. (2003). Toward understanding the psychology of reactions to perceived fairness: The role of affect intensity. Soc. Justice Res. 16: 151–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., and Bartusch, D. J. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relationship to delinquency. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103: 192–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mario Gollwitzer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gollwitzer, M., Schmitt, M., Schalke, R. et al. Asymmetrical Effects of Justice Sensitivity Perspectives on Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior. Soc Just Res 18, 183–201 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-005-7368-1

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-005-7368-1

Keywords

Navigation