Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Children’s Teleological Intuitions: What Kind of Explanations Do 7–8 Year Olds Give for the Features of Organisms, Artifacts and Natural Objects?

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research has shown that children usually provide teleological explanations for the features of organisms from a very early age (3–4 years old). However, it is not clear if teleology is applied selectively for organisms, or if it is generally applied to other objects as well (artifacts and non-living natural objects). The present study examined whether 7–8 year old students provided teleological explanations for particular organisms, artifacts and natural objects. We investigated whether children's familiarity with these objects influenced the types of explanations they gave. Finally, we also investigated correlations between 'teleology' and 'usefulness' in children's explanations. The results indicate that 7–8 year olds are able to distinguish between living and non-living entities, as well as that they provide teleological explanations mostly for organisms and artifacts. This may have important implications for biological education, since teleological explanations in classrooms are likely to pose important conceptual obstacles to the development of a scientific understanding of evolution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ariew, A. (2003). Ernst Mayr’s ‘Ultimate/Proximate’ distinction reconsidered and reconstructed. Biology and Philosophy, 18, 553–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariew, A. (2007). Teleology. In D. Hull & M. Ruse (Eds.), Cambridge companion to the philosophy of biology (pp. 160–181). Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avise, J. C. (2010). Inside the human genome: A case for non-intelligent design. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P., & Weisberg, D. S. (2007). Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science, 316, 996–997.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branch, G., & Scott, E. C. (2009). The latest face of Creationism. Scientific American, 300(1), 92–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2000). Science education as conceptual change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 13–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curry, A. (2009). Creationist beliefs persist in Europe. Science, 323, 1159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiYanni, C., & Kelemen, D. (2005). Time to get a new mountain? The role of function in children’s conceptions of natural kinds. Cognition, 97, 325–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elder, C. L. (2007). On the place of artifacts in ontology. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (pp. 33–51). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M. (2008). Conceptual change and evolutionary biology: A developmental analysis. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 263–294). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graebsch, A., & Schiermeier, Q. (2006). Anti-evolutionists raise their profile in Europe. Nature, 444, 406–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greif, M., Kemler-Nelson, D., Keil, F. C., & Guiterrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts?: Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological Science, 17(6), 455–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, R. S., & Power, B. M. (2003). The art of classroom inquiry. A handbook for teachers—researchers. Portsmouth: Heinmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kampourakis, K., & Zogza, V. (2008). Students’ intuitive explanations of the causes of homologies and adaptations. Science & Education, 17(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kampourakis, K., & Zogza, V. (2009). Preliminary evolutionary explanations: a basic framework for conceptual change and explanatory coherence in evolution. Science & Education, 18(10), 1313–1340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds and cognitive development. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1992). The origins of an autonomous biology. In M. R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in language and cognition. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 25) (pp. 103–138). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234–254). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of causal understanding of natural kinds. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 234–262). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (1999a). Function, goals and intention: children’s teleological reasoning about objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(12), 461–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (1999b). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70, 241–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (1999c). Why are rocks pointy?: children’s preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1440–1452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lennox, J. G. (1992). Teleology. In E. F. Keller & E. A. Lloyd (Eds.), Keywords in evolutionary biology (pp. 324–333). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewens, T. (2004). Organisms and artifacts: Design in nature and elsewhere. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134, 1501–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, P. C., Korthagen, F. A. J., & Vasalos, A. (2009). Supporting presence in teacher education: the connection between the personal and professional aspects of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 25(2), 297–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. D., Scott, E. C., & Okamoto, S. (2006). Public acceptance of evolution. Science, 313(5788), 765–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Numbers, R. (2009). That Creationism is a uniquely American phenomenon. In R. Numbers (Ed.), Galileo goes to jail, and other myths about science and religion (pp. 215–223). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. U. (2010). Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: II. pedagogical issues. Science & Education, 19(6–8), 539–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommers, T., & Rosenberg, A. (2003). Darwin’s nihilistic idea: evolution and the meaninglessness of life. Biology and Philosophy, 18, 653–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Southerland, S. A., Abrams, E., Cummins, C. L., & Anselmo, J. (2001). Understanding. students’ explanations of biological phenomena: conceptual frameworks or p-prims? Science Education, 85, 328–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D. (2008). Teleology. In M. Ruse (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Biology (pp. 113–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (2001/1996). Plan and purpose in nature: The limits of Darwinian evolution. London: Phoenix.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are indebted to Dimitra Tsetsou and Vassiliki Zerva for their assistance in data collection and data coding. We also thank Aggeliki Zacharaki, Dora Christodoulou and Sofia Batskini for their assistance in data collection. Finally, we are grateful to two anonymous reviewers for providing useful suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kostas Kampourakis.

Appendix I

Appendix I

 

Familiar objects

Unfamiliar objects

Questions

Duck

Scissors

Rock

Booby

Nutcracker

Stalagmite

1st question

“Have you seen this before?”

      

2nd question

“Do you know what this is?”

      

3rd question

“Is it alive or not?”

      

“Give some examples of alive and non-alive entities.” 1*

      

4th question

“Why does it have this particular feature?” 2*

Feet color

Feet shape

Color

Shape

Color

Shape

Feet color

Feet shape

Color

Shape

Color

Shape

5th question

“Is this feature useful for something?” 2*

Feet color

Feet shape

Color

Shape

Color

Shape

Feet color

Feet shape

Color

Shape

Color

Shape

  1. *1: This question was asked only with the first object (duck)
  2. *2: Questions 3 & 4 were asked consecutively each time for each feature, first for the color and then for the shape (e.g. “why does it have this color?; is this color useful for something?” and then “why does it have this shape?, is this shape useful for something?”)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kampourakis, K., Pavlidi, V., Papadopoulou, M. et al. Children’s Teleological Intuitions: What Kind of Explanations Do 7–8 Year Olds Give for the Features of Organisms, Artifacts and Natural Objects?. Res Sci Educ 42, 651–671 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-011-9219-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-011-9219-4

Keywords

Navigation