Skip to main content
Log in

Biased Judgment of Political Bias: Perceived Ideological Distance Increases Perceptions of Political Bias

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Accusations of political bias in the mass media, academia, the courts and various other institutions are common in many democracies. However, despite the prevalence of these accusations and the public attention they have received, research on the effects of perceived ideological distance on perceptions of political bias is lacking. Focusing on perceptions of political bias in academia, and drawing on a survey of 1,257 students in social science and law faculties in five Israeli universities, we show that the perceived ideological distance between a student and her set of professors increases perceptions of politically biased behavior of professors, and that the effects of ‘left-wing’ and ‘right-wing’ ideological distances are not symmetric. Possible implications and directions for further research are then suggested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Similarly, in Israel there are allegations of political bias in the media (e.g., Gur 2010), the academy (e.g., Im Tirzu 2011; Shamir 2012) and the Supreme Court (e.g., Lis 2011).

  2. For a review on the unconsciousness of many mental biases, see Wilson and Brekke (1994).

  3. For similar claims in Israel, see Kashti (2009), Im Tirzu (2011), Halutz (2012), and Shamir (2012).

  4. For a review, see Pronin et al. (2004).

  5. These were all the universities in Israel which had such departments at the time: Bar-Ilan University, Ben-Gurion University, Haifa University, Hebrew University, and Tel-Aviv University.

  6. There were four cases in which there were two cohorts at a certain department in the same year. In these cases we treated each as a distinct cohort.

  7. The theoretical relative probabilities of inclusion are thus as follows: \({\text{P}}\left( {{\text{CI|}}\overline{\text{Refusal}} } \right) > {\text{P}}\left( {{\text{CI|partial\_refusal}}} \right) > {\text{P}}\left( {\text{CI|Refusal}} \right).\)

  8. Of the remainder, 25.8 % are second-year students, 9.5 % are third-year students, and fourth- or fifth-year students compromise 1 percent of the sample.

  9. The median age of the sample is 24, whereas the real population’s median age was 24.5. The sample’s proportion of women (61.4 %) is insignificantly different from the real population’s proportion of women (59.4 %; Z = 1.43, p < .15). Data on the student population of 2009, the most recent data available for Israeli university students, was taken from the Israeli Central Bureau of Statistics (2013).

  10. The wording of all seven questions composing the Perceived Political Bias variable is shown in Appendix 1. We note that the construction of these seven items, in particular our sixth item, was complemented by the items suggested by Linvill and Havice (2011).

  11. It should be noted that while in most countries the policy content of principal axes of competition is the classical left–right, in Israel (and Turkey) the main policy dimension is the hawk vs. dove approach to regional conflicts and security (Benoit and Laver 2006). Still, comparative studies have shown that ideology in the Israeli context has similar psychological determinants (Jost et al. 2003) and political implications (e.g., vote choice: Bargsted and Kedar 2009; and policy preference formation: Sulitzeanu-Kenan and Halperin 2013) as in other countries.

  12. The first item was intended to extract from the student’s memory the total number of her professors, and in the second item the student could rely on her previous answer in verifying that she actually assigned all of her professors to the eight possible cells.

  13. We note that by doing so, we treat these ideologically unidentified professors as professors whom the students perceived as ideologically proximate. However, we suggest that perceptions of political bias in one’s professors are a structural experience (see above), in which all the professors the student is exposed to influence her learning experience. Therefore, since we asked students to evaluate the behaviors of all their professors, omitting these ideologically unidentified professors from our measure of perceived ideological distance could result in an incomprehensive measure of ideological distance. Nonetheless, in order to assess the robustness of our findings to this specification choice, a measure of perceived ideological distance in which the denominator was composed of the number of ideologically identified professors was included in our robustness tests. Our substantive findings were not found to be sensitive to this measurement choice. We also note that 104 out of 1,257 students did not assign a political position to any of their professors, and thus we could not calculate a Perceived Ideological Distance score for these students and they were excluded from our multivariate analyses. While this could somewhat bias our results, these students, as could have been expected, had much lower Perceived Political Bias scores (M = .12, SD = .10) than students who evaluated the ideological position of at least one of their professors (M = .27, SD = .17), (t(1,118) = 9.07, p < .001), and this, to a certain degree, supports our first hypothesis.

  14. For a simple intuitive demonstration consider a set of professors with no variation among their ideological positions, and assume that the student’s ideological position is identical. In this case the Perceived Ideological Distance measure will be zero. However, if the mean ideological position of the professors remains the same (and equal to that of the student) but their variance increases, the Perceived Ideological Distance will necessarily equal the professors’ ideological variance.

  15. Students in these 9 cohorts amounted to one quarter of the sample (316 out of 1,257), and they did not significantly differ from the other students in any of the demographic variables.

  16. All regression models in this paper were run with dummy variables for the different departments and universities, to account for the specific effect of each department and university on the dependent variables. In addition, in our questionnaire we also asked the students several other questions regarding their learning experience, and we varied the question order in our questionnaires, creating four questionnaire versions (see fn. 19 below). In order to account for the possible effect of the questionnaire versions, we also ran the models with dummy variables for each version but one.

  17. Perceived Ideological Distance and Ideological Position are correlated (r = −.56) but multicollinearity does not constitute a problem in this model as the variance inflation factor (VIF) of any regressor in the model is lower than 2.2.

  18. On this possible “projection” problem, see Malhotra and Jessee (2013).

  19. One tapping perceived political bias; another set tapping students’ fear to express political opinions in their professors’ presence; and a set of questions about students’ evaluations of their professors. Question order in the four versions is detailed in Sect. 2 of the Online Resource.

  20. Details on the randomization checks are available in Sect. 2 of the Online Resource.

  21. The detailed analysis is provided in Sect. 3 of the Online Resource.

  22. There were 51 students who perceived their overall professors’ ideological average position to be identical to their own ideological position. Omitting them from the right-gap group did not affect the result of the interaction term between Right-Gap and Perceived Ideological Distance in Model 4.

  23. Full results are available in Sect. 4 of the Online Resource.

  24. We thank two of the reviewers for suggesting these possibilities.

  25. Note that this asymmetry does not refer to the difference between right-wing and left-wing students, but rather between students who are exposed to views to the right of their positions, or to the left.

  26. For a broader discussion, see Smith et al. (2008, chapter 8). See also Hickey and Brecher (1990).

  27. Albeit correction of mental biases is for the most part an effortful process (Wilson and Brekke 1994).

References

  • Achen, C. H. (1978). Measuring representation. American Journal of Political Science, 22(3), 475–510.

  • American Council of Trustees and Alumni (ACTA). (2005). Intellectual diversity: Time for action. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.goacta.org/images/download/intellectual_diversity.pdf.

  • Bar, T., & Zussman, A. (2012). Partisan grading. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 4(1), 30–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargsted, M. A., & Kedar, O. (2009). Coalition-targeted Duvergerian voting: How expectations affect voter choice under proportional representation. American Journal of Political Science, 53(2), 307–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, B. L., & Johnson, C. D. (2013). On the ideological foundations of Supreme Court legitimacy in the American public. American Journal of Political Science, 57(1), 184–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauerlein, M. (2004). Liberal groupthink is anti-intellectual. Chronicle of Higher Education, November 12. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://chronicle.com/weekly/v51/i12/12b00601.htm.

  • Benoit, K., & Laver, M. (2006). Party policy in modern democracies. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2012). Identity, belief, and bias. In J. Hansin (Ed.), Ideology, psychology, and law (pp. 385–403). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L., Steele, C. M., & Ross, L. D. (1999). The mentors’ dilemma: Providing critical feedback across the racial divide. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(10), 1302–1318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Alessio, D., & Allen, M. (2000). Media bias in presidential elections: A meta-analysis. Journal of Communication, 50, 133–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Detroit Local. (2012). Santorum blasts Penn State, says grades were docked for beliefs, February 28. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://detroit.cbslocal.com/2012/02/28/santorum-tells-charlie-langton-his-college-grades-were-docked-for-his-beliefs/.

  • Dixon, J. C., & McCabe, J. (2006). Competing perspectives in the classroom: The effect of sociology students’ perceptions of “balance” on evaluations. Teaching Sociology, 34, 111–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Kulesa, P., Brannon, L. A., Shaw, K., & Hutson-Comeaux, S. (2000). Why counterattitudinal messages are as memorable as proattitudinal messages: The importance of active defense against attack. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1392–1408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eveland, W. P, Jr, & Shah, D. (2003). The impact of individual and interpersonal factors on perceived news media bias. Political Psychology, 24(1), 101–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. (2011). Partisan differences in opinionated news perceptions: A test of the hostile media effect. Political Behavior, 33, 407–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisler, J., & Foubert, J. A. (2006). Teach me, but don’t disagree with me. About Campus, 11(5), 2–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frantz, C. M. (2006). I AM being fair: The bias blind spot as a stumbling block to seeing both sides. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 157–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazal-Ayal, O., & Sulitzeanu-Kenan, R. (2010). Let my people go: Ethnic in-group bias in judicial decisions—Evidence from a randomized natural experiment. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 7(3), 403–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chaiken, S. (1994). The cause of hostile media judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 165–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, N., & Fosse, E. (2012). Why are professors liberal? Theory and Society, 41, 127–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, N., & Simmons, S. (2006). American’s views of political bias in the academy and academic freedom. Working paper. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.aaup.org/NR/rdonlyres/DCF3EBD7-509E-47AB-9AB3-FBCFFF5CA9C3/0/2006Gross.pdf.

  • Gunther, A. C. (1992). Biased press or biased public? Attitudes toward media coverage of social groups. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 147–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunther, A. C., Christen, C. T., Liebhart, J. L., & Chih, S. C.-Y. (2001). Congenial public, contrary press, and biased estimates of the climate of opinion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 65, 295–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, A. (2010). The media and the Oslo process. Tel-Aviv: Ofir Bikurim. (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2011). The bright future of post-partisan social psychology. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/haidt11/haidt11_index.html.

  • Halutz, D. (2012). Juggling act, March 1. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.haaretz.com/weekend/magazine/juggling-act-1.415769.

  • Hickey, T., & Brecher, B. (1990). In defence of bias. Studies in Higher Education, 15(3), 299–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, D. (2006). The professors: The 101 most dangerous academics in America. Washington, DC: Regnery Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Im Tirzu. (2011). Politicization at Ben-Gurion University. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.imti.org.il/Docs/P175/?ThisPageID=1278.

  • Israeli Central Bureau of Statistics. (2013). Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www1.cbs.gov.il/reader/cw_usr_view_Folder?ID=141.

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashti, O. (2009). Tel Aviv students afraid to challenge leftist professors, November 9. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.haaretz.com/print-edition/news/tel-aviv-students-afraid-to-challenge-leftist-professors-1.4537.

  • Kelly-Woessner, A., & Woessner, M. (2006). My professor is a partisan hack: How perceptions of a professor’s political views affect student course evaluations. Political Science and Politics, 39(3), 495–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly-Woessner, A., & Woessner, M. (2008). Conflict in the classroom: Considering the effects of partisan difference on political education. Journal of Political Science Education, 4(3), 265–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, K. A., & Pronin, E. (2008). When disagreement gets ugly: Perceptions of bias and the escalation of conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(6), 833–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapinski, M. K., & Boster, F. J. (2001). Modeling the ego-defensive function of attitudes. Communication Monographs, 68(3), 314–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, T.-T. (2005). The liberal media myth revisited: An examination of factors influencing perceptions of media bias. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 49(1), 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linvill, D. L. (2011). The relationship between student identity development and the perception of political bias in the college classroom. College Teaching, 59, 49–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linvill, D. L., & Havice, P. A. (2011). Political bias on campus: Understanding the student experience. Journal of College Student Development, 52(4), 487–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linvill, D. L., & Mazer, J. P. (2013). The role of student aggressive communication traits in the perception of instructor ideological bias in the classroom. Communication Education, 62(1), 48–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lis, Y. (2011). Chair of the Knesset Committee Yariv Levin: The Supreme Court endangers our ability to exist, October 1. Retrieved April 2, 2014 from http://www.haaretz.co.il/news/law/1.15158(in Hebrew).

  • Long, J. S. (1997). Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C. G., Ross, R., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Losco, J., & Deollos, I. (2007). Fear and loathing in the college classroom: A survey of political science department chairs regarding political bias. Journal of Political Science Education, 3, 251–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machina, K. (1987). Evaluating student evaluation. Academe, 73(3), 19–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malhotra, N., & Jessee, S. A. (2013). Ideological proximity and support for The Supreme Court. Political Behavior. doi:10.1007/s11109-013-9257-x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nir, L. (2011). Motivated reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 504–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, M. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 867–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological Review, 111(3), 781–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redding, R. E. (2001). Sociopolitical diversity in psychology: The case for pluralism. American Psychologist, 56(3), 205–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeder, G. D., Pryor, J. B., Wohl, M. J. A., & Griswell, M. L. (2005). On attributing negative motives to others who disagree with our opinions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1498–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, R. J., Keltner, D., Ward, A., & Ross, L. (1995). Actual versus assumed differences in construal: “Naive realism” in intergroup perception and conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 404–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naïve realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstandings. In E. S. Reed, E. Turiel, & T. Brown (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. 103–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shamir, N. (2012). Under the auspice of ‘academic freedom’ the politicization in universities strengthens. New Directions, 26, 91–106. [In Hebrew].

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. L. R., Mayer, J. D., & Fritschler, A. L. (2008). Closed minds? Politics and ideology in American universities. Washington, DC: Brooking Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sovey, A. J., & Green, D. P. (2011). Instrumental variable estimation in political science: A readers’ guide. American Journal of Political Science, 55(1), 188–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanig, P. (2013). Political polarization in retrospective economic evaluations during recessions and recoveries. Electoral Studies, 32(4), 729–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulitzeanu-Kenan, R. (2006). If they get it right: An experimental test of the effects of the appointment and reports of UK public inquiries. Public Administration, 84(3), 623–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulitzeanu-Kenan, R., & Halperin, E. (2013). Making a difference: Political efficacy and policy preference construction. British Journal of Political Science, 43(2), 295–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tollini, C. (2009). The behaviors that college students classify as political bias: Preliminary findings and implications. Teaching Sociology, 37(4), 379–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tollini, C. (2010). A comparison of faculty members’ and students’ definitions of political bias in the classroom. International Journal of Sociology and Anthropology, 2(5), 77–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsfati, Y., & Cohen, J. (2005). Democratic consequences of hostile media perceptions: The case of Gaza settlers. The Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 10(4), 28–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. (2007). The messenger overwhelming the message: Ideological cues and perceptions of bias in television news. Political Behavior, 29, 441–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallone, R. P., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. T. (1985). The hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and perception of media bias in coverage of the Beirut Massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 577–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, M. D., Domke, D., Shah, D., & Fan, D. P. (1999). Elite cues and media bias in presidential campaigns. Communication Research, 26(2), 144–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences in judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 117–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a generous grant from the Max Kampelman Chair for Democracy and Human Rights. We thank Roey Reichert, who worked with the first author on a preliminary study preceding this project. We also wish to thank Pazit Ben-nun Bloom, Chanan Cohen, Avner de-Shalit, Anat Gofen, Liat Raz-Yurovich, Ilana Ritov, Tamir Sheafer, Mario Sznajder, Gadi Wolfsfeld, the editors of Political Behavior, four anonymous reviewers, members of the Cognition and Policy Research Group at the Federmann School of Public Policy and Government at the Hebrew University, members of the Politika Forum at the Hebrew University, and participants at the annual meetings of the Israeli Political Science Association and the International Society of Political Psychology.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Ethical standards

The authors declare that this study complied with the current laws of the country in which it was performed.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Omer Yair.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOC 82 kb)

Appendix 1: The Wording of the Seven Perceived Political Bias Questions

Appendix 1: The Wording of the Seven Perceived Political Bias Questions

In your opinion, to what extent have the following statements been true about your studies this year in the Political Science Department? (1—Not at all; 2—To a small extent; 3—To a certain extent; 4—To a great extent; 5—To a very great extent)

  1. 1)

    Lecturers presented the learning material neutrally, appropriately relating to various political positions relating to the materials studied (reversed coded).

  2. 2)

    Lecturers expressed their personal political opinions on various political issues during the lessons.

  3. 3)

    As a course requirement, lecturers demanded reading of materials which presented only one political position.

  4. 4)

    Lecturers tried to convince students in classes they were teaching that a certain political position was more correct.

  5. 5)

    Lecturers were unwilling to hear political opinions which did not match their own.

To what extent do you agree with the following statements? (1—Definitely do not agree; 2—Do not agree; 3—Neutral; 4—Agree; 5—Definitely agree)

  1. 6)

    The lecturers with whom I studied in the Political Science Department tend to underestimate students who express different political attitudes than theirs during the lesson, in a paper or on a test.

  2. 7)

    The lecturers with whom I studied this year in the Political Science Department give lower marks to students who express different political attitudes than theirs during the lesson, in a paper or on a test.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yair, O., Sulitzeanu-Kenan, R. Biased Judgment of Political Bias: Perceived Ideological Distance Increases Perceptions of Political Bias. Polit Behav 37, 487–507 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9278-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9278-0

Keywords

Navigation