Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

A Light Bulb Goes On: Norms, Rhetoric, and Actions for the Public Good

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper explores whether invoking social norms, in the context of a persuasive appeal, affects individuals’ willingness to take action for the public good. The framework I develop brings together a host of factors treated as distinct in past work, including attitudes, rhetoric, and social norms. I test predictions from this framework in an experiment that focuses on a particularly important behavior—actions regarding the consumption of energy. I find that highlighting norms in the context of an appeal for energy conservation increases the importance individuals’ associate with these actions, intentions to conserve energy, and actual behavior on a light-bulb purchasing decision. The findings have implications more generally for understanding when individuals take actions that promote the public good.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Allcott (2010, p. 5) explains that “because some externalities, primarily from power plant greenhouse gas emissions, are not internalized in electricity prices, many consumers perceive that energy conservation helps provide a public good (more moderate global climate).”

  2. This is important given the debate over the validity of intention measures as predictors of behavior—e.g., literature analyzing voter turnout (see Vavreck 2007); also, see Chandon et al. (2005) for research in marketing on consumers’ purchasing choices. In a meta-review of 185 independent studies testing the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1991), Armitage and Conner (2001) find that only 19 studies contain overt measures of behavior. But see, Green and Gerber (2010), Lau and Redlawsk (2001).

  3. Whether one’s evaluation toward an object is positive, neutral, or negative depends on the availability, accessibility, and applicability of various considerations about the object (Chong and Druckman 2007).

  4. These studies find that individuals tend to systematically overestimate their own personal influence on outcomes when considering participation in a collective action. The end result of these psychological “miscalculations” is that individuals engage in collective actions at a higher rate than predicted by traditional expected-utility theory.

  5. Related work on framing effects reveals that exposure to political rhetoric affects the availability, accessibility, and applicability of cognitions toward an attitude object (Chong and Druckman 2007, 2010).

  6. Norms evolve in communities as a way to regulate social life, and norms can be especially powerful in situations in which an individual’s action causes negative effects on the lives of others (Biel and Thogersen 2007; Thogersen 2008). In these situations, norms serve to restrain egoistic impulses and induce cooperation among group members in providing public goods. For instance, research in behavioral economics on conditional cooperation demonstrates that people are more likely to contribute to the provision of public goods when they perceive others as contributing (Allcott 2010, p. 5; Alpizar et al. 2008; Axelrod 1984; Frey and Meier 2004; Shang and Croson 2004). However, individuals vary in the extent to which they regulate and control their actions when they are being monitored by others (Snyder 1987; Berinsky 2004).

  7. The four locations were: Evanston, IL; Warrenville, IL; Mount Prospect, IL; and, Lovington, IL. I transported the laptops to businesses, libraries, and community centers that generously agreed to provide a conference room to conduct the experimental sessions. Collecting data from multiple sites increased the heterogeneity of my sample; however, the location of administrative sites may have depressed the validity of some of the measurement scales described below because of increased opportunity to conserve energy in urban areas (e.g., access to public transportation).

  8. See Table 3 in the Appendix for details on the demographic composition of the sample.

  9. Subjects in the pre-test were asked to evaluate one of two versions of an editorial and whether it encourages or discourages “individuals taking personal steps to reduce energy consumption” (on a 7-point scale where 1 = “definitely opposes taking steps” and 7 = “definitely supports taking steps”). Subjects did perceive differences as to whether the editorial advocates or discourages taking personal steps to reduce energy consumption (p < .001), with the mean score for the pro-action editorial at 6.09 and the mean score for the no-action editorial at 2.74. In addition, individuals were asked to assess whether reading each editorial would decrease or increase their likelihood of conserving energy (on a 7 point scale where 1 = “definitely decreases likelihood” and 7 = “definitely increases likelihood”). The means for this question were 5.12 after reading the pro-action editorial and 3.59 after reading the editorial questioning the importance of taking personal steps (p < .001). Both articles were rated as equally easy to read and understand, and there were no differences in perceptions of how effective the editorials were in terms of making the case for conservation (means of 4.91 and 4.56 on a seven point scale for “how effective each article is in making its case”).

  10. In contrast, participants assigned to no-norms sessions read an unrelated press release. The full text of each press release is included in the Appendix.

  11. This ensures that individuals have knowledge about prevailing behavioral standards in a decision context and a belief that their expressed intentions and actions are subject to monitoring by others. I acknowledge that this may also induce social desirability effects in which respondents seek to behave in socially desirable ways (see Streb et al. 2008; Berinsky 2002; Kuklinski et al. 1997; Sniderman and Carmines 1997).

  12. These terms have been used previously by scholars to distinguish between distinct classes of energy conservation (Black et al. 1985; Stern and Gardner 1981; Stern 2000).

  13. The question was worded as follows: “A standard light bulb costs about $.50. An energy efficient light bulb is more expensive. What is the maximum amount you would be willing to pay for one energy efficient light bulb? Please enter an amount ranging from $.50 to $10.00 in the space provided below.” This protocol is an open-ended method of contingent valuation used to value environmental goods (Green et al. 1998; McFadden 1994).

  14. As a randomization check, I regressed each condition on demographic and individual-level variables including gender, education, party affiliation, political ideology, and key values measures (universalism, egalitarianism). The randomization check confirmed that these variables were unrelated to condition assignment.

  15. The full set of results for each model is reported in Tables 4, 5, and 6 in the Appendix.

  16. I used Clarify to generate predicted probability shifts across conditions (King et al. 2000; Tomz et al. 2003).

References

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behaviors. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allcott, H. (2010). Social norms and energy conservation. Typescript, MIT and NYU.

  • Alpizar, F., Carlsson, F., & Johansson-Stenman, O. (2008). Anonymity, reciprocity, and conformity: Evidence from voluntary contributions to a national park in Costa Rica. Journal of Public Economics, 92, 1047–1062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M., Jr. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychology Monograph, 70(9), 416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychology research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1985). Expectations and preferences in presidential nominating campaigns. American Political Science Review, 79, 804–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1987). Candidate choice and the dynamics of the presidential nominating process. American Journal of Political Science, 81, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1993). Messages received: The political impact of media exposure. American Political Science Review, 87, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. (2002). Silent voices: Social welfare policy opinions and political equality in America. American Journal of Political Science, 46(2), 276–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. (2004). Can we talk? Self-presentation and the survey response. Political Psychology, 25(4), 643–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biel, A., & Thogersen, J. (2007). Activation of social norms in social dilemmas: A review of the evidence and reflections on the implications for environmental behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 93–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, S. J., Stern, P. C., & Elworth, J. T. (1985). Personal and contextual influences on household energy adaptations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(1), 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T. (2011). The construction of news: Energy crises, advocacy messages, and frames toward conservation. International Journal of Press/Politics, 16(2), 143–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boninger, D. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Berent, M. K. (1995). Origins of attitude importance: Self-interest, social identification, and value relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 61–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brader, T., Nicholas, A. V., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration: Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brady, H. E., Verba, S., & Schlozman, K. L. (1995). Beyond SES: A resource model of political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 89(2), 271–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandon, P., Morwitz, V. G., & Reinartz, W. J. (2005). Do intentions really predict behavior? Self-generated validity effects in survey research. The Journal of Marketing, 69, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57, 99–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2010). Dynamic public opinion: Communication effects over time. American Political Science Review, 104(4), 663–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and Practice (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B. (2005). Basic social influence is underestimated. Psychological Inquiry, 16, 158–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98(4), 671–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2005). Media matter: How newspapers and television news cover campaigns and influence voters. Political Communication, 22, 463–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., Green, D. P., Kuklinski, J. H., & Lupia, A. (2006). The growth and development of experimental research in political science. American Political Science Review, 100(4), 627–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., Green, D. P., Kuklinski, J. H., & Lupia, A. (2011). Experimentation in political science. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Handbook of experimental political science (pp. 3–14). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Holmes, J. W. (2004). Does presidential rhetoric matter?: Priming and presidential approval. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 34, 755–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Kam, C. (2011). Students as experimental participants: A defense of the ‘narrow data base’. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Handbook of experimental political science (pp. 41–57). Boston: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2000). Preference formation. Annual Review of Political Science, 3, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Miller, J. M. (2004). The political psychology of electoral campaigns: Introduction to the symposium. Political Psychology, 25(4), 501–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Parkin, M. (2005). The impact of media bias: How editorial slant affects voters. The Journal of Politics, 67, 1030–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior? In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 204–242). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, S. E., Muller, E. N., & Opp, K.-D. (1989). Personal influence, collective rationality, and mass political action. The American Political Science Review, 83(3), 885–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B., & Meier, S. (2004). Social comparison and pro-social behavior: Testing ‘conditional cooperation’ in a field experiment. American Economic Review, 94(5), 1717–1722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2008). Social pressure and vote turnout: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102(1), 33–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2010). An experiment testing the relative effectiveness of encouraging voter participation by inducing feelings of pride or shame. Political Behavior, 32, 409–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., & Rogers, T. (2009). Descriptive social norms and motivation to vote: Everybody’s voting and so should you. The Journal of Politics, 71(1), 178–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goidel, R. K., & Nisbet, M. (2006). Exploring the roots of public participation in the controversy over embryonic stem sell research and cloning. Political Behavior, 28, 175–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goidel, R. K., & Shields, T. G. (1994). The vanishing marginals, the bandwagon, and the mass media. Journal of Politics, 56, 802–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. P., & Gerber, A. S. (2010). Introduction to social pressure and voting: New experimental evidence. Political Behavior, 32, 331–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D., Jackowitz, K. E., Kahneman, D., & McFadden, D. (1998). Referendum contingent valuation, anchoring, and willingness to pay for public goods. Resource and Energy Economics, 20, 85–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, Ed., & Kornienko, T. (2004). Running to keep in the same place: Consumer choice as a game of status. American Economic Review, 94(4), 1085–1107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoveland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 347–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. (1988). The role of attitude importance in social evaluation: A study of policy preferences, presidential candidate evaluations, and voting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(2), 196–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J. H., Cobb, M. D., & Gilens, M. (1997). Racial attitudes and the “New South”. The Journal of Politics, 59, 323–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Pomper, G. M. (2001). Effects of negative campaigning on turnout in U.S. senate elections, 1988-1998. The Journal of Politics, 63, 804–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and disadvantages of cognitive heuristics in political decision making. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 951–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubell, M., Zahran, S., & Vedlitz, A. (2007). Collective action and citizen responses to global warming. Political Behavior, 29(3), 391–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D. (1994). Contingent valuation and social choice. American Journal of Agriculture Economic, 76, 689–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2004). Threat as a motivator of political activism: A field experiment. Political Psychology, 25(4), 507–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C. (1992). Impersonal influence: Effects of representations of public opinion on political attitudes. Political Behavior, 14(2), 89–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect attitudes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nadeau, R., Cloutier, E., & Guay, J. H. (1993). New evidence about the existence of a bandwagon effect in the opinion formation process. International Political Science Review, 14(2), 202–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noelle-Neumann, E. (1984). The spiral of silence: Public opinion—our social skin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, J. P., Schultz, W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). Normative social influence is underdetected. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 913–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion: Theory and research. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. (1965). The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Gardner, R. (1992). Covenants with and without a sword. American Political Science Review, 86, 404–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quattrone, G. A., & Tversky, A. (1984). Causal versus diagnostic contingencies: On self-deception and on the voter’s illusion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 237–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenstone, S., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in America. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. P. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: A field experiment on curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 25–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. P., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2007). The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science, 18(5), 429–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shang, J., & Croson, R. (2004). Field experiments in charitable contributions: The impact of social influence on the voluntary provision of public goods. University of Pennsylvania Working paper.

  • Skalaban, A. (1988). Do polls affect elections? Some 1980 evidence. Political Behavior, 10, 136–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Carmines, E. G. (1997). Reaching beyond race. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances/private realities: The psychology of self-monitoring. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonck, N., & Loosveldt, G. (2010). Impact of poll results on personal opinions and perceptions of collective opinion. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 22, 230–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C., & Gardner, G. T. (1981). Psychological research and energy policy. American Psychologist, 36, 329–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Streb, M. J., Burrell, B., Frederick, B., & Genovese, M. A. (2008). Social desirability effects and support for a female American President. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(1), 76–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1985). Accountability: The neglected social context of judgment and choice. Research in Organizational Behavior, 7, 297–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H. (2008). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 27(1), 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thogersen, J. (2008). Social norms and cooperation in real-life social dilemmas. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 458–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomz, M., Wittenberg, J., & King, G. (2003). CLARIFY: Software for interpreting and presenting statistical results. Version 2.1. Stanford University, University of Wisconsin, and Harvard University. http://gking.harvard.edu/.

  • Vavreck, L. (2007). The exaggerated effects of advertising on turnout: The dangers of self-reports. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 2, 325–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origin of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank Dennis Chong, Fay Lomax Cook, James N. Druckman, Jennifer Jerit, Martin Johnson, Peter Loewen, Sean Richey, and Kim Mannemar Sonderskon for comments on this manuscript. I thank Lindsey Herbel and Veronica Luckow for research assistance. The research was supported by a Graduate Research Grant from Northwestern University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Toby Bolsen.

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 3, 4, 5, and 6.

Table 3 Demographic profile of the sample
Table 4 Treatment effects on attitudes and reported intentions
Table 5 Determinants of behavioral intentions
Table 6 Determinants of purchasing behavior

Press Release—Pro-Behavior Norm

Cook County Residents Committed to Reducing Energy Consumption

Researchers at Northwestern University have released the results of a study on energy attitudes among Cook County residents. One of the more notable findings is the considerable extent to which residents are willing to take steps to reduce personal energy consumption. For example, the vast majority of respondents said that the next time they buy a car they will consider energy efficient alternatives, and most respondents said that they have, or intend to buy, energy-efficient light bulbs. Also, nearly 90% reported that it is “very important” that all Americans purchase energy efficient light bulbs even though they are more costly. Full details of the study are available at www.northwestern.edu/cookcounty/energysurvey.

Press Release—Control Group (No Norm)

Cook County Residents Say Economy is the Most Important Issue Facing Nation

Researchers at Northwestern University have released the results from a study on issues important to Cook County residents in the upcoming presidential election. Similar to the results from national polls, 36% of respondents said that the general economic situation is the most important issue facing the nation. The situation in Iraq was the second most frequently cited problem with 20% of the sample volunteering this response. Other issues cited as important included immigration policy, energy and fuel costs, and health care costs. Full details of the study are available at www.northwestern.edu/cookcounty/electionsurvey.

Editorial (Pro Action—Consumers)

Editorial: Your Choices about Energy Consumption Matter

Energy is essential to the economic activity that sustains and improves the quality of our lives. But in the last 25 years, world energy demand has increased about 60%, raising questions about how to address the increased energy usage.

Some argue that the government needs to take responsibility. Others say the responsibility lies more with individuals’ consumption decisions. Along these lines, a recent report from McKinsey Global Organization found that the growth rate of worldwide energy consumption could be cut substantially through more aggressive energy-efficiency efforts by households. For instance, switching from familiar, incandescent light bulbs to longer-lasting, energy-saving compact fluorescent bulbs would save consumers billions of dollars annually and save the world from millions of metric tons of greenhouse gases. Compact fluorescent light bulbs are only slightly more costly than conventional bulbs, yet consume 75% less electricity. “This is not a sacrifice deal,” Roger Walker, head of Cambridge Energy Research Associates, says of energy conservation. “This is a technology deal. After all, we’re twice as energy efficient now as we were in the 1970s, and this increase in efficiency is largely a result of consumers’ actions aimed at reducing energy costs. We must continue to urge consumers to make energy efficient choices.”

In July of 2007, the National Petroleum Council released the results of a 2-year study commissioned by U.S. Energy Secretary Samuel Bodman. One of five core strategies the report lists to assist markets in meeting energy challenges for 2030 and beyond involves reducing energy demand by increasing the efficiency of transportation and residential energy uses. Thus, consumers will play an increasingly important role in U.S. energy policy in the coming decades. Whether these polices are effective depends on our ability to reduce energy consumption.

Editorial (Pro Action—Government)

Editorial: Government Must Take Action to Achieve Energy Independence

Energy is essential to the economic activity that sustains and improves the quality of our lives. But in the last 25 years, world energy demand has increased about 60%, raising questions about how to address the increased energy usage.

Some argue that individuals should take steps to reduce energy consumption, while others say the government needs to take responsibility. According to the latter group, only government has the capacity to coordinate the actions of millions of individuals and businesses to ensure a stable, reliable energy supply. For instance, a strong financial commitment to research and development of alternative energy sources, such as renewable energies and energy-efficient technologies, might one day free the U.S. from dependence on oil. But the transition from a primarily fossil-fuel-based society to a renewable-energy-society will take decades and require government leadership. Voluntary reductions in personal energy consumption will have a relatively small impact on the nation’s overall energy usage. For example, decisions consumers make about light bulb and appliance purchases will do little to foster a transition away from a reliance on energy produced from burning fossil fuels. Nonetheless, recent energy legislation includes regulations that will actually cost consumers more money in the short term. To assuage voters, politicians hide the price tag when they try to impose conservation. The efficiency standards for appliances, far from paying for themselves, will cost consumers roughly $50 billion through 2050, according to Roger Walker, head of Cambridge Energy Research Associates. Not only do these measures cost consumers a lot, but they also do little to nothing in terms of affecting the nation’s energy situation.

Instead of mandating that individuals pay more for household products and appliances, a successful long-term approach to energy security requires expanding and diversifying energy production by investing in cost–effective energy technologies, providing a reliable energy infrastructure, and funding research and development for environmentally-clean ways to utilize the nation’s vast domestic coal reserves. Thus, it is government, not consumers, who must commit to taking action if we hope to achieve real energy independence.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bolsen, T. A Light Bulb Goes On: Norms, Rhetoric, and Actions for the Public Good. Polit Behav 35, 1–20 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9186-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9186-5

Keywords

Navigation