Skip to main content
Log in

Embodying gestures: The Social Orienting Model and the study of early gestures in autism

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Autistic spectrum disorders impair the ability to interact socially. Detecting and understanding their onset is not only an empirical enterprise, but also a theoretical one, often linked to studies on intersubjectivity. Different theoretical perspectives have been elaborated in the past to account for the deficit. The main purpose of this paper is to reinforce and offer empirical grounding to a recent approach, termed Social Orienting Model, by presenting the main theoretical approaches to autism and contrasting them to this view, as well as considering its possible effect on empirical research, focusing on current literature analyzing gestures in children with autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This list does not in any way wish to cover all the available theoretical literature on autism, but rather to point out the main lines of current research. Readers interested in a historical review the concept of autism and its relation to other deficits may find one in Wing 2005.

  2. A similar line of reasoning may be held for the Central Coherence approach and the Emotional approach, which have been linked to one or the other of the theories described above.

  3. To this point Goffman has observed that participants in a social interaction accord different weights to each other’s behavior and that individuals are able to parse out what he terms an ‘attentional track’, i.e. within social communication there are always certain acts that are treated as being relevant vs. others that are judged as being incidental (Goffman 1974). Kendon states that the way in which behavior is parsed and relevance of meaning is ascribed to actions is consistent among individuals and that while the number of gestures may vary their quality displays many similarities (Kendon 2005).

References

  • Argyle, M. (1972). Non-verbal communication in human social interaction. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal communication. London: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, A., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and down’s syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(2), 241–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of Ag. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of autism: evolutionary approaches. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1249–1257, 2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’. Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bello, A., Capirci, O., & Volterra, V. (2004). Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: spontaneous use of gesture in naming task. Neuropsychologia, 42, 201–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogdan, R. (1997). Interpreting minds: the evolution of a practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, T. G. R. (1972). Object perception in infants. Perception, 1, 15–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1974). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language & Communication, 1(2/3), 155–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: a reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96(1), 168–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C. (1983). Communication to language: deaf children’s and hearing children’s development compared. Sign Language Studies, 39, 113–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., longobardi, E., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with down syndrome. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 41, 1125–1135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, L., Fabri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., et al. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. PNAS, 104(45), 17825–17830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Autism in infancy and early childhood. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 223–246). New Jersey: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. A. (1978). The transition from action to gesture. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 231–257). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colgan, E. S., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., & Baranek, G. T. (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouththe origins of language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dautenhahn, K. (2001). The narrative intelligence hypothesis: In search of the transactional format of narratives in humans and other social animals. In M. Beynon, C. L. Nehaniv, & K. Dautenhahn (Eds.), Proceedings of the fourth international cognitive technology conference, CT2001: Instruments of mind (pp. 248–266). Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M., Dautenhahn, K., Nehaniv, C. L., & Powell, S. D. (2007). The narrative construction of our (social) world: steps towards an interactive learning environment for children with autism. Universal Access in the Information Society, 6, 145–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 399–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in Rhesus Macaques. Plos Biology, 4(9), 1501–1508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., & Ferrari, P. F. (2004). Mirror neurons, gestures and language evolution. Interaction Studies, 5(3), 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, S., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Genetics of autism: Complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 2, 943–955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or primary interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 83–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2004). Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 11(3), 199–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). Phenomenological contributions to a theory of social cognition. Husserl Studies, 21, 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2007). Simulation trouble. Social Neuroscience, 2(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. (2006). Primary interaction and narrative practice. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C., Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Meltzoff, A. (1996). The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau–Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology, 9, 213–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2001). The ‘shared manifold’ hypothesis. From mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2006). Corpo vivo, simulazione incarnata e intersoggettività. Una prospettiva neurofenomenologica. In M. Cappuccio (Ed.), Neurofenomenologia, Le scienze della mente e la sfida dell’esperienza cosciente. Milano: Bruno Mondadori.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, S., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation in the inferior parietal lobule. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance (vol. 19, pp. 247–266). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2(12), 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., Fogassi, L., Luppino, G., Mattelli, M., Camarda, R., & Rizzolatti, G. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey: I. Somatotopy and the control of proximal movements. Experimental Brain Research, 71, 475–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Butcher, C. (2003). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. (1989). Interpretation psychologized. Mind and Language, 4, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating minds. The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mind reading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. M. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language, 1, 158–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. M., & Cruz, J. (2003). Simulation theory. In Encyclopedia of cognitive science (vol. 4, pp. 9–14). London: The Nature Publishing Group/Macmillan Reference Ltd.

  • Gray, H. (1978). Learning to take an object from the mother. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language (pp. 159–182). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grezès, J., Armony, J. L., Rowe, J., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Activations related to mirror and canonical neurons in the human brain: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 18, 928–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heal, J. (1986). Replication and functionalism. In J. Butterfield (Ed.), Language, mind and logic (pp. 135–150). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. J. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. J. (2005). Autism and emotion. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 406–425). New Jersey: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunctions in autism. Neurophsychologia, 32, 477–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2003). Folk-psychological narratives and the case of autism. Philosophical Papers, 33(3), 345–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2004). The limits of spectatorial folk-psychology. Mind and Language, 19, 548–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2006). Narrative practice and understanding reasons: Reply to Gallagher. In R. Menary (Ed.), Consciousness and emotion: Special issue on radical enactivism (pp. 231–248).

  • Hutto, D. D. (2007). Folk Psychology without theory or simulation. In D. D. Hutto & M. Ratcliffe (Eds.), Folk psychology reassessed. Doredrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (2003). Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 179–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain. The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 19–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Wozniak, R. H. (2007). Variation in vocal-motor development in infant siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 158–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2005). Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation and mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (2002). Gesture and language dialectic. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia. Available at http://www.semioticon.com/virtuals/talks/gesture_language.pdf.

  • McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture & thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1999). Persons and representation: why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Markus, J. (2004). On the nature of communication and language impairment in autism. In C. M. Shore (Ed.), The many faces of childhood (p. 54). New Jersey: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Pineda, J. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J. K., & Noe, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Broken mirrors. A theory of autism. Scientific American, 295, 62–69, November.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhea, P., & Sutherland, P. (2005). Enhancing early language in children with autism spectrum disorders. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (p. 946). New Jersey: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricks, D. M. (1972). The beginning of verbal communication in normal and autistic children. M.D. thesis, London.

  • Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5(3), 191–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, M., Luppino, G., & Mattelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey: II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research, 71, 491–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American, 295, 54–61, November.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2006). So quel che fai. Il cervello che agisce e i neuroni specchio. Milano: Raffaello Cortina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. (2001). Diagnosis of autism before the age of 3. In L. G. Masters (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (vol. 23, pp. 1–31). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 631–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, P. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheler, M. (1923). Wesen und Formen der Sympathie. Bonn: Verlag von Friedrich Cohen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, O. (2004). Narrative introductions: discourse competence of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Discourse Studies, 6(2), 253–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Hoffman, E. L., Lewis, S. E., & Ousley, O. Y. (1994). Early recognition of autism. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 148, 174–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year-old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(6), 677–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O. B., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. (2002). Infantile reflexes gone astray in autism. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 6, 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Mureri, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 13982–13987 November.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (p. 182). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., & Pizzuto, E. (2005). Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In M. Tomasello & D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Beyond nature-nurture. Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, L. R., Baranek, G. T., Crais, E. R., Hughes, C., Kristof, M. L., & Zanzot, E. (2000). Gesture use of infants with autism at 9–12 months, poster session presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Assosiation Convention, November 17, Washington D.C.

  • Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G., & Brian, A. A. (1989). Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: Implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 48–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs. Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (2005). Problems of categorical classification systems. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (p. 583). New Jersey: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D., & Parnas, J. (2003). Conceptual problems in infantile autism research. Why cognitive science needs phenomenology. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(9–10), 53–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral markers of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neurosciences, 23, 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Thurm, A., Stone, W., Baranek, G., Bryson, S., Iverson, I., et al. (2007). Studying the emergence of autism spectrum disorders in high-risk infants: methodological and practical issues. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 466–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Sparaci.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sparaci, L. Embodying gestures: The Social Orienting Model and the study of early gestures in autism. Phenom Cogn Sci 7, 203–223 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9084-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9084-9

Keywords

Navigation