Skip to main content
Log in

Another’s punishment cleanses the self: Evidence for a moral cleansing function of punishing transgressors

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Separate lines of research show that individuals: (a) understand immorality metaphorically as physical contamination; (b) project undesirable self-attributes onto others; and (c) view punishment as eliminating a transgressor’s immorality. Integrating these findings, we hypothesized that individuals project guilt over their own immorality—represented as physical contamination—onto another transgressor whose punishment restores their own moral and physical purity. In Study 1, personal immorality salience decreased felt physical cleanliness unless another transgressor was punished. In Study 2, personal immorality salience led participants to see another transgressor as physically dirtier, an effect mediated by guilt. Furthermore, the punishment of the contaminated transgressor restored participants’ personal morality and eliminated restorative moral behavior. In Study 3, punishing a transgressor who served as a projection target for participants’ immorality removed felt physical contamination indirectly through decreased guilt. These studies are the first to show that another’s punishment can “cleanse” the self of “dirty” immorality feelings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is also worth noting that these findings provide what is, to our knowledge, the most direct evidence to date that people can feel soiled by their own moral violations. Zhong and Liljenquist (2006) indirectly assessing participant’s felt physical contamination by measuring the accessibility of cleaning-related words (Study 1), or willingness to pay more for cleaning products (Study 2). The current study directly assessed participants’ self-reported feelings of personal physical cleanliness.

  2. Data for 9 participants were excluded from all analyses for failing to correctly identify that the topic of the news article was a hit-and-run car accident.

  3. In an effort to increase the internal reliability of our non-moral negative trait measure, for Study 2 we selected three negative traits that were all related to the target transgressor’s perceived competence.

  4. This interaction remained significant when controlling for participants’ comparative rankings of their standing on morally-irrelevant positive traits (p = .03).

  5. To ensure the validity and reliability of our findings we included a series of key attention check items following the two articles describing the transgression targets. These items required participants to identify (a) the nature of the transgressions described in the articles, (b) whether or not the transgressor described in the article was punished, and, (c) whether or not both articles described the same transgressor or different transgressors.

  6. Submitting participants’ BJW scores to a 2 (personal immorality salience) × 2 (punishment target) ANOVA did yield a marginal two-way interaction, F(1, 280) = 3.22, p = .07, η 2p  = .01. However, in contrast to the pattern of effects on guilt and personal physical dirtiness scores, significant differences in BJW scores only emerged for participants in the immorality-not-salient condition. Specifically, pairwise comparisons revealed that when immorality was not made salient, participants exposed to the punishment of the projection target reported significantly greater BJW scores (M = 3.74, SD = .93) than those exposed to the punishment of a non-projection target (M = 3.33, SD = .99). In contrast, when immorality was made salient, BJW scores did not differ between those exposed to the punishment of the projection target (M = 3.56, SD = .95) and those exposed to the punishment of the non-projection target (M = 3.56, SD = .96).

  7. We also tested a reversed mediated moderation analysis but switching the mediator (guilt) and outcome variable (personal physical dirtiness). The 95 % confidence interval obtained for the indirect effects of immorality salience × punishment target interaction on guilt through perceived personal dirtiness scores through guilt did not contain zero (−.20, −.02).

References

  • Adams, G. S. (2011). Punishers become more deviant (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from http://purl.stanford.edu/yz983kw3263.

  • Adams, G. S., & Mullen, E. (2014). Punishing the perpetrator decreases compensation for victims. Social and Personality Science, 6, 31–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, F., & Staub, H. (1956). The criminal, the judge, and the public: A psychological analysis. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1979). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. (Original work published 1954).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., Jetten, J., & Fasoli, F. (2011). Cleansing the soul by hurting the flesh: The guilt-reducing effect of pain. Psychological Science, 22, 334–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 232–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323, 1179–1180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalbert, C., Montada, L., & Schmitt, M. (1987). Glaube an eine gerechte Welt as Motiv: Validierungskorrelate zweier Skalen [Belief in a just world as motive: Concurrent validation of two scales]. Psychologische Beiträge, 4, 596–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darlington, R. B., & Macker, C. E. (1966). Displacement of guilt-produced altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 442–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, T. (1995). Scapegoats: Transferring blame. New York: Routledge Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of prison (A. Sheridan, Trans.). New York: Random House Inc.

  • Frazer, J. G. (2002). The golden bough (Abridged ed.). New York: Macmillan. (Original work published 1922).

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, A. (1936). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. London: Hogarth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1957). Instincts and their vicissitudes. In J. Rickman (Ed.), A general selection from the works of Sigmund Freud. New York: Liveright. (Original work published 1915).

    Google Scholar 

  • Govorun, O., Fuegen, K., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Stereotypes focus defensive projection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 781–793.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 613–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, J. (1977). Projection: A test of the psychoanalytic hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 536–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, D. S., & Houston, B. K. (1971). The defensive function of projection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20, 208–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizzaro, D. A., Gilovich, T., & Ariely, D. (2013). Moral masochism: On the connection between guilt and self-punishment. Emotion, 13, 14–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. J., Cheung, F., & Donnellan, M. B. (2014). Does cleanliness influence moral judgments? A direct replication of Schnall, Benton, and Harvey (2008). Social Psychology, 45, 209–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1968). Analytical psychology: Its theory and practice (the Tavistock lectures). New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaspar, K. (2013). Washing one’s hands after failure enhances optimism but hampers future performance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 69–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. K. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychology bulletin, 136, 1045–1067.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010a). Dirty hands and dirty mouths: Embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression. Psychological Science, 21, 1423–1425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010b). Washing away postdecisional dissonance. Science, 328, 709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2011). Wiping the slate clean: Psychological consequences of physical cleansing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 307–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 737–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, T. E., Cohen, A., Shahin, L. K., Malov, S., Golan, Y., & Busnach, S. (2014). Being clean and acting dirty: The paradoxical effect of self-cleansing. Ethics & Behavior. doi:10.1080/10508422.2014.931230.

  • Merritt, A. C., Effron, D. A., & Monin, B. (2010). Moral self-licensing: When being good frees us to be bad. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 344–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignacio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Daquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 68–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. M. A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2009). When guilt evokes self-punishment: Evidence for the existence of a Dobby effect. Emotion, 9, 118–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Regan, D. T., Williams, M., & Sparling, S. (1972). Voluntary expiation of guilt: A field experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 42–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothschild, Z. K., Landau, M. J., Molina, L. E., Branscombe, N. R., & Sullivan, D. (2013). Displacing blame over the ingroup’s harming of a disadvantaged group can fuel moral outrage at a third-party scapegoat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 898–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Lowery, L., & Ebert, R. (1994a). Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 870–881.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & McCauley, C. (1994b). Sensitivity to indirect contacts with other persons: AIDS aversion as a composite of aversion to strangers, infection, moral taint and misfortune. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 495–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Martens, A. (2003). Evidence that projection of a feared trait can serve a defensive function. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 969–979.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008a). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 1219–1222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008b). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1096–1109.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomarken, A. J., & Serlin, R. C. (1986). Comparison of ANOVA alternatives under variance heterogeneity and specific noncentrality structures. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 90–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Victor, G. (2003). Scapegoating—A ritual of purification. Journal of Psychohistory, 30(3), 271–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, A. J., Zwick, R., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Washing away your (good or bad) luck: Physical cleansing affects risk-taking behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 26–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. A. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B., Strejcek, B., & Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 859–862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zachary K. Rothschild.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rothschild, Z.K., Landau, M.J., Keefer, L.A. et al. Another’s punishment cleanses the self: Evidence for a moral cleansing function of punishing transgressors. Motiv Emot 39, 722–741 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9487-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9487-9

Keywords

Navigation