Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Older but wiser: Age moderates congruency effects between implicit and explicit motives on job satisfaction

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the light of an aging workforce, age differences in workers’ motives are important guidelines for an age-differentiated human resource management. Whereas research has addressed age differences in explicit work values or motives, age differences in implicit motives and in the congruency between implicit and explicit motives (i-e-congruency) have been neglected so far. In two studies (N = 201 and 751), we investigated chronological age as a moderator of the relationship between i-e-congruency and job satisfaction. In general, we expected that high i-e-congruency is positively related to job satisfaction. Moreover, life-experience and a change in future-work time-perspective should lead to higher i-e-congruency for older than for younger workers. Finally, we hypothesized that the relationship between i-e-congruency and job satisfaction is moderated by workers’ age such that i-e-congruency is more strongly related to job satisfaction for older than for younger workers. Results supported our hypotheses in the affiliation motive domain. Implications for workers’ careers and an age differentiated human resource management are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The malleability and development of traits and trait-like concepts is extensively discussed in the literature. Personality traits do not only develop (gradually) over the life-span but may also change as a result of major life events (see for example Specht et al. 2011). The same is true for explicit (work) values that are relatively stable but may be amended to maintain a positive self-concept (Rokeach 1973). Changes in work values may be evoked by events such as entering the workforce or job change, as well the mere presence or absence of rewards (Johnson 2001). Hence, we would argue that the current way of conceptualizing motives as trait-like concepts that may nevertheless be changed (gradually) is well in line with established theories and empirical evidence.

  2. We excluded 6 participants due to missing data in PSE stories.

  3. As the number of indicators for each implicit motive was significantly correlated with the number of words (affiliation: r = .61, p < .001; achievement:, r = .40, p < .001; power: r = .50, p < .001), the motive imagery score was corrected for number of words by using regression analysis, and standardized regression residuals were used as motive imagery scores in all subsequent analyses (Schultheiss and Pang 2007).

  4. We did not include moderation terms between age and squared implicit and explicit motives because they are not part of our theoretical model (cf. Edwards and Parry 1993). Please note, inclusion of these terms did not change the general data pattern.

  5. As recommended by Shanock et al. (2010), we inspected how many participants would be considered to have discrepancies between implicit and explicit motives, i.e., participants with a standardized implicit motive score that is half a standard deviation above or below the standardized score of the explicit motive. Thus, we calculated three agreement groups, whether workers show a stronger explicit motive (35.32 % for affiliation, 37.81 % for achievement, and 37.31 % for power), show equally strong motives (29.85, 28.86, and 28.86 %, respectively), or show a stronger implicit motive (34.83, 33.33, and 33.83 %, respectively). Based on our data, we conclude that exploring i-e-congruency makes practical sense.

  6. We excluded 118 participants who either completed the questionnaire very fast according to pretests (i.e., participants who constituted outliers in the distribution of time to complete the survey, who were identified based on a scree plot) or crossed always the same box throughout the questionnaire.

  7. It is important to note, that the MMG allows the assessment of both, a hope and a fear component of implicit motives (e.g., hope-of-affiliation and fear-of-rejection). However, our research question refers to the hope component of the implicit motive. Thus, we did not include fear motives into our model (cf. Schüler 2010; Schüler et al. 2008, 2009).

  8. We calculated three agreement groups, whether workers show a stronger explicit motive (36.22 % for affiliation, 35.95 % for achievement, and 33.82 % for power), show equally strong motives (27.96, 30.01, and 29.16 %, respectively), or show a stronger implicit motive (35.82, 33.95, and 37.02 %, respectively). Based on our data, we conclude that exploring i-e-congruency makes practical sense.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 781–799. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2010). Implicit motives: A look from personality systems interaction theory. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 375–403). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bowling, N. A., Eschleman, K. J., & Wang, Q. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between job satisfaction and subjective well-being. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 83, 915–934. doi:10.1348/096317909X478557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C. (2010). Implicit motives and explicit goals: The role of motivational congruence in emotional well-being. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 3–29). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cable, D. M., & DeRue, D. S. (2002). The convergent and discriminant validity of subjective fit perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 875–884. doi:10.1037//0021-9010.87.5.875.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312, 1913–1915. doi:10.1126/science.1127488.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 117–121. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Devloo, T., Anseel, F., & De Beuckelaer, A. (2011). Do managers use feedback seeking as a strategy to regulate demands-abilities misfit? The moderating role of implicit person theory. Journal of Business and Psychology, 26, 453–465. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9200-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards (2011, August 9). Moderated polynomial regression. Retrieved from http://public.kenanflagler.unc.edu/faculty/edwardsj/ModeratedPolynomialRegression.htm.

  • Edwards, J. R., & Parry, M. E. (1993). On the use of polynomial regression equations as an alternative to difference scores in organizational research. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 1577–1613. doi:10.2307/256822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.80.3.501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 74, 175–222. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00373.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199–223. doi:10.1023/A:1025007614869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Göritz, A. S., Wolff, H.-G., & Goldstein, D. G. (2008). Individual payments as a longer-term incentive in online panels. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 1144–1149. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.4.1144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffeth, R., Hom, P., & Gaertner, S. (2000). A meta-analysis of antecedents and correlates of employee turnover: Update, moderator tests, and research implications for the next millennium. Journal of Management, 26, 463–488. doi:10.1177/014920630002600305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, J. W., & Borman, W. C. (2012). The oxford handbook of work and aging. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, G., van der Heijden, B., De Lange, A., & Deller, J. (2013). Facilitating age diversity in organizations—part I: Challenging popular misbeliefs. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 28, 729–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, J., & Chasiotis, A. (2003). Congruence of life goals and implicit motives as predictors of life satisfaction: Cross-cultural implications of a study of Zambian male adolescents. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 251–272. doi:10.1023/A:1025011815778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, J., Chasiotis, A., & Campos, D. (2006). Congruence between social values and implicit motives: Effects on life satisfaction across three cultures. European Journal of Personality, 20, 305–324. doi:10.1002/per.590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices. A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, D. N. (1984). Personality research form manual. Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Get a taste of your goals: Promoting motive goal congruence though affect-focus goal fantasy. Journal of Personality, 77, 1527–1559. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00591.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Oertig, D., Brandstätter, V., & Allemand, M. (2010). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit motivation and unhealthy eating behavior. Journal of Personality, 78, 1209–1238. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00648.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K. (2001). Change in job values during the transition to adulthood. Work and Occupations, 28, 315–345. doi:10.1177/0730888401028003004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2011). Directional discrepancy between implicit and explicit power motives is related to well-being among managers. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 317–327. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9219-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehr, H. M. (2002). Souveränes Selbstmanagement. Ein wirksames Konzept zur Förderung von Motivation und Willensstärke. [Self-management: An effective concept to enhance motivation and will-power] Weinheim: Beltz.

  • Kehr, H. M. (2004a). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and perceived abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and volition. Academy of Management Review, 29, 479–499. doi:10.2307/20159055.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehr, H. M. (2004b). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327. doi:10.1177/0146167203256967.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Köllner, M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2012). Implicit and explicit motive measures: A meta-analysis of their correlation and potential moderators of the relationship. Paper presented at the 16th European Conference on Personality, Trieste.

  • Kooij, D. T. A. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., Kanfer, R., & Dikkers, J. S. E. (2011). Age and work-related motives: Results of a meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 197–225. doi:10.1002/job.665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristof-Brown, A. L., & Guay, R. P. (2011). Person-environnent fit. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), American psychological association handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1–50). Chicago: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumm, S., Grube, A., & Hertel, G. (2013). No time for compromises: Age as a moderator of the relation between needs-supply fit and job satisfaction. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22, 547–562. doi:10.1080/1359432X.2012.676248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decomposing self-regulation and self-control: The volitional components inventory. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 15–49). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 201–206. doi:10.1046/j.0963-7214.2003.01262.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989a). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425–437. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.4.4.425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., DeVoe, M., & Schoeberlein, S. (1989b). Emotions and self-regulation: A life span view. Human Development, 32, 279–299. doi:10.1159/000276480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, J. W. B., Zettler, I., Ewen, C., & Hülsheger, U. R. (2012). Implicit motives, explicit traits, and task and contextual performance at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 1201–1217. doi:10.1037/a0029556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1297–1349). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., et al. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment: A review of evidence and issues. American Psychologist, 56, 128–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. P., Stanley, D. J., Herscovitch, L., & Topolnytsky, L. (2002). Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: A meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 61, 20–52. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2001.1842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic apperception test. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuberger, O., & Allerbeck, M. (1978). Messung und Analyse von Arbeitszufriedenheit. Erfahrungen mit dem “Arbeitsbeschreibungsbogen (ABB)” [Measurement and analysis of job satisfaction. Experiences with the ‘Job Description Questionnaire’]. Bern: Huber.

  • Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2010). The relationships of age with job attitudes: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 63, 677–718. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2011). Age and motivation predict gaze behavior for facial expressions. Psychology and Aging, 26, 695–700. doi:10.1037/a0023281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U. S. college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 280–294. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8503_04.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, Y.-J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2010). Motive training and motivational competence. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 510–548). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. Ney York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schattke, K., Koestner, R., & Kehr, H. M. (2011). Childhood correlates of adult levels of incongruence between implicit and explicit motives. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 306–316. doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9182-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2000). Zum gegenwärtigen Stand der Motivdiagnostik [The current state of motvation diagnostics]. Diagnostica, 46, 115–123. doi:10.1026//0012-1924.46.3.115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437–453. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1987.tb00609.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J. (2007). Arousal of flow experience in a learning setting and its effects on exam performance and affect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 21, 217–227. doi:10.1024/1010-0652.21.3.217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J. (2010). Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 2–14. doi:10.1007/s11031-009-9150-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J., Brandstätter, V., & Sheldon, K. (2013). Do implicit motives and basic psychological needs interact to predict well-being and flow? Testing a universal hypothesis and a matching hypothesis. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 480–495. doi:10.1007/s11031-012-9317-2.

  • Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2008). A high implicit affiliation motive does not always make you happy: A corresponding explicit motive and corresponding behavior are further needed. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 231–242. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9096-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Dealing with a ‘hidden‘stressor: Emotional disclosure as a coping strategy to overcome negative effects of motive incongruence on health. Stress and Health, 25, 221–233. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9096-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J., Sheldon, K. M., & Fröhlich, S. M. (2010). Implicit need for achievement moderates the relationship between competence need satisfaction and subsequent motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 603–633). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7701_05.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2010). Implicit motives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 322–344). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Patalakh, M., Rawolle, M., Liening, S., & MacInnes, J. J. (2011). Referential competence is associated with motivational congruence. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 59–70. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.11.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikvo, B., & Schad, D. J. (2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test using the Picture Story Exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 72–81. doi:10.1080/00223890802484456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shanock, L. R., Baran, B. E., Gentry, W. A., Pattison, S. C., & Heggestad, E. D. (2010). Polynomial regression with response surface analysis: A powerful approach for examining moderation and overcoming limitations of difference scores. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25, 543–554. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9183-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Integrating behavioral-motive and experiential-requirement perspectives on psychological needs: A two process model. Psychological Review, 118, 552–569. doi:10.1037/a0024758.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Schüler, J. (2011). Wanting, having, and needing: Integrating motive disposition theory and self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1106–1123. doi:10.1037/a0024952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H.-D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: The Multi-Motiv-Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126–145. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA740109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Specht, J., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2011). Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 862–882. doi:10.1037/a0024950.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stumpf, H., Angleitner, A., Wieck, T., Jackson, D. N., & Beloch-Till, H. (1985). Deutsche Personality Research Form (PRF) [German Personality Research Form (PRF)]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thielgen, M. M., Krumm, S., & Hertel, G. (2014). When being old pays off: Age mitigates adverse effects of low implicit—explicit motive congruency on work motivation. Journal of Career Assessment. doi:10.1177/1069072714547613.

  • Thrash, T. M., Cassidy, S. E., Maruskin, L. A., & Elliot, A. J. (2010). Factors that influence the relation between implicit and explicit motives: A general implicit-explicit congruence framework. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 308–346). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed achievement motives: Concordance and predictive validity. Journal of Personality, 70, 729–755. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.05022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thrash, T. M., Elliot, A. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). Methodological and dispositional predictors of congruence between implicit and explicit need for achievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 961–974. doi:10.1177/0146167207301018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Torelli, C. J., & Shavitt, S. (2010). Culture and concepts of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 703–723. doi:10.1037/a0019973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E. L., Leavitt, K., Menges, J. I., Koopman, J., Howe, M. D., & Johnson, R. E. (2012). Getting explicit about the implicit: A taxonomy of implicit measures and guide for their use in organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 15, 553–601. doi:10.1177/1094428112442750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Beek, I., Taris, T. W., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2011). Workaholic and work engaged employees: Dead ringers or worlds apart? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16, 468–482. doi:10.1037/a0024392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

  • Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2009). Remaining time and opportunities at work: Relationships between age, work characteristics, and occupational future time perspective. Psychology and Aging, 24, 487–493. doi:10.1037/a0015425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG) awarded to Guido Hertel (He 2745/11-3).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus M. Thielgen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thielgen, M.M., Krumm, S., Rauschenbach, C. et al. Older but wiser: Age moderates congruency effects between implicit and explicit motives on job satisfaction. Motiv Emot 39, 182–200 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9448-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9448-8

Keywords

Navigation