Skip to main content
Log in

For which side the bell tolls: The laterality of approach-avoidance associative networks

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The two hemispheres of the brain appear to play different roles in emotion and/or motivation. A great deal of previous research has examined the valence hypothesis (left hemisphere = positive; right = negative), but an increasing body of work has supported the motivational hypothesis (left hemisphere = approach; right = avoidance) as an alternative. The present investigation (N = 117) sought to provide novel support for the latter perspective. Left versus right hemispheres were briefly activated by neutral lateralized auditory primes. Subsequently, participants categorized approach versus avoidance words as quickly and accurately as possible. Performance in the task revealed that approach-related thoughts were more accessible following left-hemispheric activation, whereas avoidance-related thoughts were more accessible following right-hemispheric activation. The present results are the first to examine such lateralized differences in accessible motivational thoughts, which may underlie more “downstream” manifestations of approach and avoidance motivation such as judgments, decision making, and behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassel, C., & Schiff, B. B. (2001). Unilateral vibrotactile stimulation induces emotional biases in cognition and performance. Neuropsychologia, 39, 282–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonezzi, A., Brendl, C., & De Angelis, M. (2011). Stuck in the middle: The psychophysics of goal pursuit. Psychological Science, 22, 607–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P. (1982). Laterality: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 839–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1984). Hemispheric asymmetry and emotion. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 320–365). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1992). Emotion and affective style: Hemispheric substrates. Psychological Science, 3, 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1999). Neuropsychological perspectives on affective styles and their cognitive consequences. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 103–123). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake, R. A. (1987). Conceptions of own versus others’ outcomes: Manipulation by monaural attentional orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 373–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eder, A. B., & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 262–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlichman, H. (1984). Methodological issues in lateral eye movement research. Current status of lateral eye movement research. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto.

  • Elliot, A., Maier, M., Moller, A., Friedman, R., & Meinhardt, J. (2007). Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 154–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804–818.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, M., Hassin, R., & Bargh, J. (2008). Implicit motivation: Past, present, and future. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 150–166). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). Automaticity in goal pursuit. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 624–646). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84, 451–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Sigelman, J. D., Bohlig, A., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2003). Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effect of coping potential on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrington, J. D., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Engels, A. S., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., et al. (2010). Localization of asymmetric brain function in emotion and depression. Psychophysiology, 47, 442–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., & Hicks, R. E. (1978). Functional cerebral space: A model for overflow, transfer and interference effects in human performance: A tutorial review. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VII (pp. 345–362). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1045–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malamed, E., & Larsen, B. (1977). Regional cerebral blood flow during voluntary conjugate eye movements in man. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 56(Suppl. 64), 530–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Emotion as motion: Asymmetries in approach and avoidant actions. Psychological Science, 18, 1113–1119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 207–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D. (2004). Personality as performance: Categorization tendencies and their correlates. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 127–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D. (2007a). Lives lived in milliseconds: Using cognitive methods in personality research. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 345–359). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D. (2007b). Gassing, braking, and self-regulating: Error self-regulation, well-being, and goal-related processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., & Compton, R. J. (2008). The happy mind in action: The cognitive basis of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 220–238). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., Meier, B. P., Tamir, M., Wilkowski, B. M., & Ode, S. (2009). Behavioral facilitation: A cognitive model of individual differences in approach motivation. Emotion, 9, 70–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., Moeller, S. K., & Ode, S. (2010). Extraversion and reward-related processing: Probing incentive motivation in affective priming tasks. Emotion, 10, 615–626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Catching the mind in action: Implicit methods in personality research and assessment. In M. Eid & E. Diener (Eds.), Handbook of multimethod measurement in psychology (pp. 115–125). Washington, DC: American Psychology Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., Ode, S., Moeller, S. K., & Goetz, P. W. (2007). Neuroticism and affective priming: Evidence for a neuroticism-linked negative schema. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1221–1231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, R. G., & Price, T. R. (1982). Post-stroke depressive disorders: A follow-up study of 103 patients. Stroke, 13, 635–641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., & Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, B. B., & Bassel, C. (1996). Effects of asymmetrical hemispheric activation on approach and withdrawal responses. Neuropsychology, 10, 557–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seibt, B., Neumann, R., Nussinson, R., & Strack, F. (2008). Movement direction or change in distance? Self- and object-related approach-avoidance motions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 713–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., & Solberg, E. (2006). You may worry, but can you recognize threats when you see them?; Neuroticism, threat identifications, and negative affect. Journal of Personality, 74, 1481–1506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomarken, A. J., Davidson, R. J., Wheeler, R. E., & Doss, R. C. (1992). Individual differences in anterior brain asymmetry and fundamental dimensions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 676–687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This publication was made possible by COBRE Grant P20 RR020151 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam K. Fetterman.

Additional information

Its contents are the sole responsibility of the authors and do not necessary reflect the official views of NCRR or NIH.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fetterman, A.K., Ode, S. & Robinson, M.D. For which side the bell tolls: The laterality of approach-avoidance associative networks. Motiv Emot 37, 33–38 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9306-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9306-5

Keywords

Navigation