Skip to main content
Log in

Actual environments do affect motivation and psychological adjustment: A test of self-determination theory in a natural setting

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the impact of the actual environment on changes in psychological adjustment over time. According to Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci and Ryan, Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior, 1985a, Plenum, New York; J Res Pers 19:109–134, 1985b; Psychol Inq 11:227–268, 2000), environments that are objectively supportive of autonomy should facilitate psychological adjustment through their impact on people’s subjective perceptions of autonomy and self-determined motivation. The present study tested this hypothesis using a prospective design with nursing homes residents. Results from structural equation modeling showed that actual autonomy-supportive nursing home environments were positively associated with residents’ perceptions of autonomy that in turn predicted self-determined motivation in major life domains. Self-determined motivation, in turn, predicted increases in psychological adjustment over a one-year period. Theoretical implications of the present findings are discussed in line with SDT.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Another approach would have been to measure residents’ self-determined motivation twice––at Times 1 and 2. However, the length of the scale was a clear obstacle with such a type of population, especially knowing that the attrition rate is high and that the sample size is usually small. We wanted the maximum number of participants to take part in the second phase of the study. Therefore, we decided to use a short questionnaire at Time 1 and opted for the method of a one-year time interval between residents’ perceptions of autonomy and self-determined motivation assessments.

  2. Because we wanted to assess the influence of nursing homes’ autonomy support on the motivational sequence at the between level of the nursing homes, we were interested in the between covariance of the model variables. We expected that the variance in nursing homes autonomy support between the nursing homes would lead to perceptions of autonomy at the mean average of the residents living in each nursing homes that would in turn lead to residents’ mean average of self-determined motivation and residents’ mean average of increases in psychological adjustment. Variation at the within level, that is variation among residents of a same nursing home, was also plausible because of individual differences—not all participants were equally self-determined or equally psychologically adjusted when entering their respective nursing home. Thus, these variations were expected and they were predicted to vary in the same direction of the hypothesized between level model (it was not the present research goal to examine differences between the nursing homes level [between level] and the residents level [within level]). Results from two separate path analyses using between and within (pooled) covariance matrices showed that model fit indices were excellent for both between and within models (p s > .35; RMSEA = 0.00, CFI > .95, SRMR < .05), thus suggesting that there was variance to be modeled at both levels (between and within). However, the between level model had to be computed from 11 nursing homes (groups = n = 11). Therefore, not all parameter estimates were significant at p < .05 (they were at p one-tailed < .10) (see Hox and Maas 2001 for an estimation of the bias in parameter estimates with a small group sample size in a multilevel structural equation analysis). Because both covariance matrices fit adequately the hypothesized model, it was hypothesized that they should vary in the same direction, but independently from each other. If such was the case, they could thus be combined on this basis. To test this model of invariance, we examined concurrently the entire model composed of the between and within covariances using a multilevel structural equation modeling technique (see Hox 2002; Stapleton 2006). In this analysis, both matrices (between and within) are modeled together according to the hypothesized model and are expected to vary independently and to be equal—that is, each parameter estimate and measurement error at the within level is constrained to be equal to its corresponding one at the between level. Fit indices for this model were excellent, χ2(10) = 11.69, p = .31, CFI = .96, GFI = .97, RMSEA = .042, thus suggesting that the same hypothesized model held at both the between and within levels. Because both matrices were independent and adequately and equally fit the model, we thus computed a path analysis with the whole variance (i.e., without separating between and within variances) in order to evaluate the significance of the parameter estimates and provide mediation analyses with a sufficient power to detect them. The details of this path analysis are presented in the Results section.

  3. The hypothesized path between psychological adjustment and perceptions of autonomy is equivalent to a covariance (see Hershberger 2006; Kline 2005 on equivalent models). However, because psychological adjustment at Time 1 is used as a control variable to remove shared variance with psychological adjustment at Time 2, it needs to be modeled as an exogenous variable in the model (psychological adjustment at Time 1 is not predicted by any variable). Hence, it is not well advised to have a covariance between an exogenous and endogenous variable in a structural equation model (because the measurement error of an exogenous variable is fixed to 1.00 for identification purposes). Thus, it has been replaced with a direct path. It should be understood that psychological adjustment at Time 1 is not meant to be seen as predicting perceptions of autonomy, but is only correlated with it (a covariation). However, such a covariation is in line with past research that has shown that psychological adjustment is associated with greater perceptions of autonomy (Deci et al. 2001).

  4. Controlling for residents’ daily required number of care hours or residents’ health level as rated by head nurses did not change the results. An alternative model was also tested. This alternative model tested the sequence “Actual Autonomy-Supportive Environment → Perceptions of Autonomy → Changes in Psychological Adjustment → Self-Determined Motivation”. Results revealed poor fit indices for this model, χ 2 (df = 4, = 83) = 8.53, p = .07, RMSEA = .119, thus suggesting that the original hypothesized model should be preferred. Other potential alternative models were not theoretically or methodologically possible, thus they were not tested.

  5. Baron and Kenny’s classical step 1—that is, to assess the correlation between the predictor and the outcome—was dropped as simulation studies have shown that this step is not a necessary condition (Mackinnon et al. 2002), particularly with distal effect (such as in a prospective study) and with small sample sizes (Shrout and Bolger 2002).

References

  • Asher, H. B. (1976). Causal modeling. New York: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need satisfaction: A motivational basis of performance and well-being in two work settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2045–2068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 57–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blais, M. R., Sabourin, S., Boucher, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (1990). Toward a motivational model of couple happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1021–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1980). The psychology of self-determination. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath (Lexington Books).

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985a). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and the “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern bloc country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 930–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in elementary and junior high school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 257–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children’s learning: An experimental and individual difference investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 890–898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent style associated with children’s self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). The inner resources for school performance: Motivational mediators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 508–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grouzet, F. M. E., Vallerand, R. J., Thill, E. E., & Provencher, P. J. (2004). From environmental factors to outcomes: A test of motivational causal sequence. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hershberger, S. L. (2006). The problem of equivalent structural models. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 13–42). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis. Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J., & Maas, C. J. M. (2001). The accuracy of multilevel structural equation modeling with pseudobalanced groups and small samples. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 157–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jopp, D., & Rott, C. (2006). Adaptation in very old age: Exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes for centenarians’ happiness. Psychology and Aging, 21, 266–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2003). LISREL 8.54 for Windows (Computer software). Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (1996). Distinguishing reactive versus reflective autonomy. Journal of Personality, 64, 465–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways of being highly motivated: A closer look at introjection, identification, and intrinsic motivation. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984) Setting limits on children’s behavior: The differential effects of controlling vs. informational styles on intrinsic motivation and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niemiec, C. P., Lynch, M. F., Vansteenkiste, M., Bernstein, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). The antecedents and consequences of autonomous self-regulation for college: A self-determination theory perspective on socialization. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 761–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, B. P., & Vallerand, R. J. (1994). Motivation, self-determination, and person-environment fit as predictors of psychological adjustment among nursing home residents. Psychology and Aging, 9, 189–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, B. P., & Vallerand, R. J. (1998). Psychological adjustment variables as predictors of mortality among nursing home residents. Psychology and Aging, 13, 368–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, G. L., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Brière, N. M. (2001). Associations between perceived autonomy support, forms of self regulation, and persistence: a prospective study. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratelle, C. F., Vallerand, R. J., Chantal, Y., & Provencher, P. J. (2004). Cognitive adaptation and mental health: A motivational analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 459–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratelle, C. F., Vallerand, R. J., Senecal, C., & Provencher, P. J. (2005). The relationship between school-leisure conflict and educational and mental health indexes: A motivational analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1800–1823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, D. W., Haas, G., & Hawkings, D. (1977). Locus of desired control and positve self-concept of the elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 32, 441–450.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reker, G. T., Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. (1987). Meaning and purpose in life and well-being: A life-span perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 44–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richard, J. F., & Schneider, B. H. (2005). Assessing friendship motivation during preadolescence and early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 367–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NH: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). Autonomy, relatedness, and the self: Their relation to development and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and methods (Vol. 1, pp. 618–655). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319–1331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stapleton, L. M. (2006). Using multilevel structural equation modeling techniques with complex sample data. In G. R. Hancock & R. Mueller (Eds.), A second course in structural equation modeling (pp. 415–445). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, A. B., MacKinnon, D. P., & Tein, J. Y. (2008). Tests of the three-path mediated effect. Organization Research Methods, 11, 241–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 271–360). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60, 599–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: Toward a motivational model of high school dropout. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1161–1176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., & O’Connor, B. P. (1989). Motivation in the elderly: A theoretical framework and some promising findings. Canadian Psychology, 30, 538–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., & O’Connor, B. P. (1991). Construction et validation de l’Échelle de Motivation pour les Personnes Agées (EMPA) [Construction and validation of the Elderly Motivation Scale (EMS)]. International Journal of Psychology, 26, 219–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., O’Connor, B. P., & Hamel, M. (1995). Motivation in later life: Theory and assessment. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 41, 221–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 37–63). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolk, S., & Telleen, S. (1976). Psychological and social correlates of residential constraint. Journal of Gerontology, 31, 89–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On the importance of self-determination for intrinsically motivated behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert J. Vallerand.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Philippe, F.L., Vallerand, R.J. Actual environments do affect motivation and psychological adjustment: A test of self-determination theory in a natural setting. Motiv Emot 32, 81–89 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9087-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9087-z

Keywords

Navigation