Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional and motivational reactions to failure: the role of illusions of control and explicitness of feedback

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research has found that illusions of control (IOC) can buffer against the emotional consequences of failure and may increase persistence in the face of failure. Theoretical analyses suggest that IOC are most closely related to a better mood and higher motivation if failure feedback is open to reinterpretation. Two experiments were conducted to test the hypothesis that explicitness (vs. ambiguousness) of failure feedback moderates the consequences of IOC following a failure experience. In accord with predictions, it was found that IOC were subsequently related to a better mood (Study 1) and higher persistence (Study 2) if individuals received ambiguous task-inherent failure feedback. In contrast, explicit failure feedback seemed to neutralize the beneficial effects of IOC. These results are discussed with respect to the adaptiveness of IOC.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A pilot study found that the non-contingent conditions elicited higher ratings of perceived control when some of the fields in the matrix were colored.

  2. In an additional hierarchical regression analyses, mood at T3 was regressed on IOC and the experimental conditions (Step 1) and the interaction of IOC and experimental conditions (Step 2) without controlling for mood at T1. Compared to the analysis which controlled for mood at T1, this analysis yielded similar, although somewhat weaker results. In Step 1, the experimental conditions significantly predicted mood at T1, b = −.19, se b = .08, t(204) = 2.32, P < .05. In Step 2, the interaction of predictors significantly predicted mood at T3, b = −.21, se b = .08, t(203) = 2.53, P < .05. IOC were positively related to mood at T3 in the task-inherent feedback condition, r = .35, P < .001. IOC and mood at T3 were unrelated in the explicit feedback condition, r = −.09, ns. The two correlations were significantly different, Z = 3.22, P < .001.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Clements, C. M. (1992). Illusions of control: Invulnerability to negative affect and depressive symptoms after laboratory and natural stressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 234–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., Kelly, K. A., Mineka, S., & Clements, C. M. (1990). Comorbidity in anxiety and depressive disorders: A helplessness-hopelessness perspective. In J. D. Maser, & C. R. Cloninger (Eds.), Comorbidity in anxiety and mood-disorders (pp. 499–543). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset in behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 86–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Great expectations: Optimism and pessimism in achievement settings. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 239–255). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham, J. T., & Alicke, M. D. (2002). The influence of individual versus aggregate social comparison and the presence of others on self-evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1117–1130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M. (1985). Desire for control and achievement-related behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1520–1533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and self-regulation. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 31–51). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P., Cohen, J., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/ correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003). Why we thought that action mind-sets affect illusions of control. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 261–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusions of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 531–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heltzer, R., & Vyse, S. A. (1994). Intermittent consequences and problem solving: The experimental control of “superstitious” beliefs. Psychological Record, 44, 155–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langens, T. A. (2007). Regulatory focus and illusions of control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langens, T. A., & Schüler, J. (2007). Effects of written emotional expression: The role of positive expectancies. Health Psychology, 26, 174–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B. A., & Ritov, I. (2004). The affective costs of overconfidence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 281–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, F. P. (1993). It won’t happen to me: Unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? British Journal of Psychology, 84, 39–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 910–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M. (1989). Cognitive interference and learned helplessness: The effects of off-task cognitions on performance following unsolvable problems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 129–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M. (1994). Human learned helplessness: A coping perspective. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puca, R. M. (2004). Action phases and goal setting: Being optimistic after decision masking without getting into trouble. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 121–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, R. W., & Beer, J. S. (2001). Positive illusions about the self: Short-term benefits and long-term costs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 340–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B. Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self (vol. 2, pp. 33–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effect of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 213–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist, 55, 99–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. C. (2004). Illusions of control. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions (pp. 113–126). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. C., Armstrong, W., & Thomas, C. (1998). Illusions of control, underestimation and accuracy: A control heuristics explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 143–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

I’d like to thank Jennifer Fett, Aneta Kozlowski, Rainer Lehmann, Susanne Reinsberg, Tatjana Schlaht, and Lisa Warmbier for their in help in data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas A. Langens.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Langens, T.A. Emotional and motivational reactions to failure: the role of illusions of control and explicitness of feedback. Motiv Emot 31, 105–114 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9058-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9058-9

Keywords

Navigation