Skip to main content
Log in

The Induction of Mood via the WWW

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Five experiments explored the methodology of Web-based mood induction and yielded empirical evidence for its feasibility and its limits. Study 1 examined the suitability of the Velten method and mood-suggestive photographs to induce both positive and negative moods. Negative mood was successfully induced, while the positive mood induction failed. In Study 2, cartoons successfully ameliorated mood, whereas jokes were ineffective. In Study 3, associations with affectively valenced words were not suitable in producing a positive or a negative mood. In Study 4, picture-illustrated emotive texts successfully induced a positive and a negative mood. In Study 5, valence and arousal ratings were obtained for the stimuli used in Study 1 and 3. Based on the results, recommendations for the application of online mood induction procedures are given.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In a pre-test, four male and two female judges rated the photographs on a 6-point scale with anchors in the following order: clearly mood-lifting, slightly mood-lifting, mood-neutral, slightly depressing, clearly depressing, and shocking. For ethical reasons, 12 photographs were excluded which at least three judges found shocking. From the remaining photographs, the 58 pictures with the lowest mean score were used for the positive and the 57 with the highest score for the negative mood induction.

  2. Several measures were taken to ensure that participants only took part once. In Studies 1, 3, and 4, and in part in Study 2 participants were recruited from an online panel: They could access the pages with a personal password only. In Study 2 there were also ad hoc recruited participants. All datasets were sifted according to IP-number and timestamps. Therefore, it can be assumed that very few, if any, multiple participations remained undetected.

  3. Mean effect sizes are based on my own re-analysis of Westermann et al.’s (1996) data. To obtain mean effects, each individual effect size was Fisher-transformed and weighted by sample size (Hedges & Olkin, 1985, pp. 230–231).

  4. Larsen and Sinnett (1991) did not differentiate between negative and positive induction.

  5. A sensitivity analysis showed that the effects were invariant no matter how strictly the criteria for inclusion were set.

References

  • Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the internet. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 803–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report C-1, Gainesville, FL. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

  • Carnevale, P. J. D., & Isen, A. M. (1986). The influence of positive affect and visual access on the discovery of integrative solutions in bilateral negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R. (1985). Choosing among multiple categories: The effect of mood on category accessibility, inference, and affect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pittsburg, PA: Carnegie-Mellon University.

  • Festinger, L. (1962). Cognitive dissonance. Scientific American, 207, 93–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1998). Asking nicely? The effects of mood on responding to more or less polite requests. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behaviour. Psychological Inquiry, 13(1), 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. O., & Green, M. L. (1982). Velten mood induction procedure effects: Duration and postexperimental removal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 341–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Göritz, A. S. (2004). Recruitment for online access panels. International Journal of Market Research, 46, 411–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Göritz, A. S. (in press). Using online panels in psychological research. In A. Joinson, K. McKenna, T. Postmes, & U.-D. Reips (Eds.), Oxford handbook of internet psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Göritz, A. S., & Moser, K. (2006). Web-based mood induction. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 887–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling S. D., Vazire S., Srivastava S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should We Trust Web-Based Studies? A Comparative Analysis of Six Preconceptions About Internet Questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. (1999). Internet addiction: Fact or fiction? Psychologist, 12, 246–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampel, R. (1977). Adjektiv-Skalen zur Einschätzung der Stimmung (SES) [Adjective scales to assess mood]. Diagnostica, 23, 43–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B. (1978). Effects of altruism on mood. Journal of Social Psychology, 102, 197–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawking, S. (1997). Die Illustrierte Kurze Geschichte der Zeit [The illustrated A brief history of time]. Reinbek, Germany: Rowohlt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Gorgoglione, J. M. (1983). Some specific effects of four affect-induction procedures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 136–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 20–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesow, R. M. (2002). Überwachen und Rächen. Todesstrafe live [Surveilling and avenging. Death penalty live]. Kursbuch, 147, 30–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, A. (1986). Die Auswirkungen emotionaler Zustände auf das Lösen eines sozialen Dilemmas [The effects of emotional states on the solving of a social dilemma]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 17, 160–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Sinnett, L. M. (1991). Meta-analysis of experimental manipulations: Some factors affecting the Velten mood induction procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 323–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linden, W., & Frankish, J. (1988). Expectancy and type of activity: Effects on pre-stress cardiovascular adaptation. Biological Psychology, 27, 227–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. Z., Solomon, M. R., & Jaffe, R. H. (1980). Weight differences in emotional responsiveness to proprioceptive and pictorial stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 308–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, K. O., Tew, M. D., & Williams, J. E. (2000). The integrity of Web-delivered experiments: Can you trust the data? Psychological Science, 11, 502–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Novak, T. D., Hoffman, D. L., & Yung, Y. F. (2000). Measuring the customer experience in online environments: A structural modeling approach. Marketing Science, 19, 22–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popp, M. (1988). Wirkung von Informationsbedingung, emotionalem Zustand und externer Speichermöglichkeit auf das Lösen sozialer Fallen [Effect of information condition, emotional state, and possibility of external storage on the solving of social traps]. Archiv für Psychologie, 140, 33–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (2005). Why emotion names and experiences don’t neatly pair. Psychological Inquiry, 16, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shavit, T., Sonsino, D., & Benzion, U. (2001). A comparative study of lotteries-evaluation in class and on the Web. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 483–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verheyen, C., & Göritz, A. S. (2003). Text-Based Online Mood Induction. Final thesis by first author, supervised by second author. University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Department of Organizational and Social Psychology.

  • Wefing, H. (2002). Die Lebenskandidaten. Wendet Amerika sich wirklich von der Todesstrafe ab? [Candidates for life. Does America really turn away from capital punishment?] Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, 26.6.2002, 145, p. 45.

  • Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, G. M., & Clark, M. S. (1989). Providing help and desired relationship type as determinants of changes in moods and self-evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 722–734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, W. F., & Bower, G. H. (1992). Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 276–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work has been supported by a University of Erlangen-Nürnberg post-doctoral scholarship (HWP). I thank Klaus Moser for helpful comments on two of the studies presented in this paper and for providing the institutional backdrop for all five studies.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anja S. Göritz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Göritz, A.S. The Induction of Mood via the WWW. Motiv Emot 31, 35–47 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9047-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9047-4

Keywords

Navigation