Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of synchronizing consumers' diurnal preferences with time of response on data reliability

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Daily rhythms of arousal explain a broad range of variations in biological, behavioral, and cognitive patterns among humans. In this paper, we consider the effects of individual variations in optimal time-of-day on the stability of consumer responses. Specifically, we explore the effects of gathering questionnaire responses at times of day that match (versus mismatch) consumers' temporal preferences on data reliability. Two studies were conducted using two different scales. Our findings confirm a synchrony effect between participants' testing time and test–retest reliability results. Specifically, we found greater (lower) stability in test–retest scores for surveys administered during times-of-day matching (mismatching) consumers' temporal preferences. We discuss theoretical and practical implications and opportunities for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For all Alpha's we have also calculated their confidence intervals using the computation procedure suggested by Iacobucci and Duhachek (2003).

  2. The following formula was used, throughout the paper, to calculate the magnitude of all experimental effects (effect size): \( r = \left( {\text{eta}} \right) = \sqrt {\frac{{(F)\left( {df\,{\text{effect}}} \right)}}{{(F)\left( {df\,{\text{effect}} + \left( {df\,{\text{error}}} \right)} \right)}}} . \) Rosenthal and Rosnow (1984, p. 356).

References

  • Andershed, A. (2005). In synchrony with adolescence: The role of morningness-eveningness in development. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, A., Tarone, R., Sandler, D., & Lynch, C. (2001). Reliability of reporting on life-style. Epidemiology (Cambridgem, Mass.), 13(1), 94–99. doi:10.1097/00001648-200201000-00015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, V. G. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1(5), 319–322. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, A., & Harrison, M. C. (1979). A test of the reliability of psychographics. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(1), 32–38. doi:10.2307/3150871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clancy, K., & Ostlund, L. (1976). Commercial effectiveness measures. Journal of Advertising Research, 16(1), 26–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1987). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutt-Bergman, M. (2004). A descriptive narrative of healthy lasting using psychographics. Health Marketing Quarterly, 20(3), 81–101. doi:10.1300/J026v20n03_06.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fasssnacht, M. (2009). The death of consumer segmentation? Rethinking a traditional marketing tool. Advertising Age, (April), 13.

  • Freeman, G., & Hovland, C. (1934). Diurnal variation in performance and related physiological processes. Psychological Bulletin, 31, 777–799. doi:10.1037/h0071917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, D., Hahn, C., & Hasher, L. (2007). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 431–440. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & May, C. P. (2005). It’s about time: Circadian rhythms, memory, and aging. In I. Chizuko & N. Ohta (Eds.), Human learning and memory: Advances in theory and application. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, A., Massey, P., & Cronan, T. (1993). On the test-retest reliability of perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use scales. MIS Quarterly, 13(3), 227–231. doi:10.2307/249803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, C. O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness–eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4(1), 97–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornik, J. (1988). Diurnal variations in consumer response. The Journal of Consumer Research, 14(4), 588–592. doi:10.1086/209139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hornik, J. (1990). Time orientation psychographics, and smoking behavior. Journal of Health Care Marketing, 10(1), 36–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacobucci, D., & Duhacheck, A. (2003). Advancing alpha: Measuring reliability with confidence. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(4), 478–487. doi:10.1207/S15327663JCP1304_14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A., & Pierro, A. (2008). Night and day, you are the one: On circadian mismatches and the transference effect in social perception. Psychological Science, 19(3), 296–301. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02083.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lalwani, A. K. (2009). The distinct influence of cognitive busyness and need for closure on cultural differences in socially desirable responding. The Journal of Consumer Research, 36(3) (in press).

  • Lastovika, J. (1982). On the validity of life-style tarits: A review and illustration. JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 19(1), 126–138. doi:10.2307/3151537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, G., & Davies, R. (2001). Individual differences in energetic arousal and sustained attention: A dual-task study. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(3), 575–589. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00162-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 363–379. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElroy, T., & Mosteller, L. (2006). The influence of circadian type, time of day and class difficulty on students' grades. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 4(3), 611–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKelvie, S. J. (1992). Does memory contaminate test-retest reliability? The Journal of General Psychology, 119(1), 59–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnelly, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ottenbacher, K. (1995). An examination of reliability in developmental research. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16, 177–182. doi:10.1097/00004703-199506000-00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peter, P. J. (1979). Reliability: A review of psychometric basics and recent marketing practices. Journal of Marketing Research, 16(1), 6–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plocher, T., Goonetilleke, R., Yan, Z., & Liang, S. -F. M. (2002). Time orientation across cultures. In: J. Coronado, D. L. Day, B. Hall (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th Annual International Workshop on Internationalisation of Products and Systems (IWIPS), (pp. 23–31). Austin, Texas, USA

  • Roehm, H. A., & Roehm, M. L. (2004). Variety-seeking and time of day: Why leader brands hope young adults shop in the afternoon, but follower brands hope for morning. Marketing Letters, 15(4), 213–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1984). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, A. G., & Ball, D. A. (1981). Statistical power and effect size in marketing research. JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 28, 275–290. doi:10.2307/3150969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 420–428. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, A. J. (1977). Test-retest correlations and the reliability of copy testing. JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 14(3), 476–486. doi:10.2307/3151188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taillard, J., Philip, P., & Chastang, J. (2004). Validation of horne and ostberg morningness–eveningness questionnaire. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 19(1), 76–86. doi:10.1177/0748730403259849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. E. (1996). Toward a psychological theory of multidimensional activation (Arousal). Motivation and Emotion, 2(1), 1–33. doi:10.1007/BF00992729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villani, K. E. A., & Lehmann, D. (1975). An examination of the stability of AIO measures. In E. M. Muzze (Ed.), Marketing: The challenges and the opportunities (pp. 484–88). Chicago: American Marketing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, W. D. (1975). Psychographics: A critical review. Journal of Marketing Research, 12(2), 196–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, C. (1997). Age differences in consumers processing strategies: An investigation of moderating influences. The Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 329–340. doi:10.1086/209514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, C., May, C. P., Goldstein, D., & Hasher, L. (2000). Aging, circadian arousal patterns and cognition. In D. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive Aging: A Primer (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jacob Hornik.

Additional information

This research was financially supported by the Henry Crown Center for Business Research, Israel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hornik, J., Tal, A. The effect of synchronizing consumers' diurnal preferences with time of response on data reliability. Mark Lett 21, 1–15 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-009-9080-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-009-9080-7

Keywords

Navigation