Skip to main content
Log in

Identifying Infants and Toddlers at High Risk for Persistent Delays

  • Published:
Maternal and Child Health Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives Little is known about the extent to which a developmental delay identified in infancy persists into early childhood. This study examined the persistence of developmental delays in a large nationally representative sample of infants and toddlers who did not receive early intervention. Methods In a sample (n ≈ 8700) derived from the early childhood longitudinal study, birth cohort, we examined developmental changes between 9 and 24 months. Motor and cognitive delays were categorized as none, mild, and moderate/severe. Adjusted ordinal logistic regression models estimated the likelihood of worse developmental delay at 24 months. Results About 24 % of children had a cognitive delay and 27 % had a motor delay at either 9- or 24-months. About 77 % of children with mild and 70 % of children with moderate/severe cognitive or motor developmental delay at 9-months had no delay at 24-months. Children with mild cognitive delay at 9-months had 2.4 times the odds of having worse cognitive function at 24-months compared to children with no cognitive delay at 9 months. Children with moderate/severe cognitive delay at 9-months had three times the odds of having worse cognitive abilities at 24-months than children who had no cognitive delay at 9-months. Similar results were found for motor skills. Conclusions Developmental delays in infants are changeable, often resolving without treatment. This work provides knowledge about baseline trajectories of infants without and without cognitive and motor delays. It documents the proportion of children’s delays that are likely to be outgrown without EI and the rate at which typically-developing infants are likely to display developmental delays at 2-years of age.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

ECLS-B:

Early childhood longitudinal study, birth cohort

EI:

Early intervention

BSF-R:

Bayley short form-research edition

BSID-II:

Bayley scales of infant development, second edition

IRT:

Item response theory

References

  1. American Academy of Pediatrics. (2006). Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: An algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics, 118, 405–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bailey, D. B., Hebbeler, K., Scarborough, A., Spiker, D., & Mallik, S. (2004). First experiences with early intervention: A national perspective. Pediatrics, 113, 887–896.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Aylward, G. P. (2009). Developmental screening and assessment: What are we thinking? Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 169–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rosenberg, S. A., Robinson, C. C., Shaw, E. F., & Ellison, M. C. (2013). Part C early intervention for infants and toddlers: Percentage eligible vs. served. Pediatrics, 131, 38–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Simpson, G. A., Colpe, L., & Greenspan, S. (2003). Measuring functional developmental delay in infants and young children: prevalence rates from the NHIS-D. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 17, 68–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rosenberg, S. A., Zhang, D., & Robinson, C. C. (2008). Prevalence of developmental delays and participation in early intervention services for young children. Pediatrics, 121, 503–1509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Hebbeler, K., Spiker, D., Bailey, D., et al. (2007). Early intervention for infants and toddlers with disabilities and their families: Participants, services, and outcomes. In Final report of the National Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (Internet) (cited 2011 Nov 3). http://www.seels.net/

  8. Cheng, E. R., Palta, M., Kotelchuck, M., Poehlmann, J., & Witt, W. P. (2014). Cognitive delay and behavior problems prior to school age. Pediatrics, 134(3), e749–e757.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hillemeier, M. M., Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., & Maczuga, S. A. (2011). Perinatal and socioeconomic risk factors for variable and persistent cognitive delay at 24 and 48 months of age in a national sample. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 15, 1001–1010.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nord, C., Edwards, B., Andreassen, C., et al. (2006). Early childhood longitudinal study, birth cohort (ECLS-B), user’s manual for the ECLS-B longitudinal 9-month: 2-year data file and electronic codebook (NCES2006-046). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Andreassen, C., & Fletcher, P. (2005) Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, birth cohort (ECLS-B), psychometric characteristics. In Volume 1 of the ECLS-B methodology report for the nine-month data collection (NCES 2005-100). Washington, DC: US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

  12. Bayley, N. (1993). Manual for the Bayley Scales of infant development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Individuals with disabilities education improvement act of 2004, Part C. 34 CFR Part 303, Department of Education, Section 303.21(a)(2)(ii). http://idea.ed.gov/part-c/downloads/IDEA-Regulations.pdf. Accessed on July 1, 2014.

  14. McManus, B. M., Magnusson, D., & Rosenberg, S. A. (2013). Restricting state Part C eligibility policy is associated with lower early intervention utilization. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 18, 1031–1037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. McManus, B. M., McCormick, M. C., Acevedo-Garcia, D., et al. (2009). The effect of state early intervention (EI) eligibility on EI participation among children with special health care needs. Pediatrics, 124, S368–S374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied logistic regression (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  17. SAS v9.13. Copyright 2002–2003. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC

  18. Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2008). School outcomes social-emotional screening status in early childhood predicts elementary. Pediatrics, 121, 957.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. McManus, B. M., & Poehlmann, J. (2011). Maternal depression and perceived social support as predictors of cognitive function trajectories during the first 3 years of life for preterm infants in Wisconsin. Child: Care, Health and Development,. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01253.x.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Colombo, J., Carlson, S. E., Cheatham, C. L., Fitzgerald-Gustafson, K. M., Kepler, A., & Doty, T. (2011). Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infancy reduces heart rate and positively affects distribution of attention. Pediatric Research, 70, 406–410.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hutchison, A. K., Hunter, S. K., Wagner, B. D., Calvin, E. A., Zerbe, G. O., & Ross, R. G. (2013). Diminished infant P50 sensory gating predicts increased 40-month-old attention, anxiety/depression, and externalizing symptoms. Journal of Attention Disorders,. doi:10.1177/1087054713488824.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Messinger, D., Lambert, B., Bauer, C. R., Bann, C. M., Hamlin-Smith, K., & Das, A. (2010). The relationship between behavior ratings and concurrent and subsequent mental and motor performance in toddlers born at extremely low birth weight. Journal of Early Intervention, 32, 214–233.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Canals, J., Hernández-Martínez, C., Esparó, G., & Fernández-Ballart, J. (2011). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale as a predictor of cognitive development and IQ in full-term infants: a 6-year longitudinal study. Acta Paediatrica, 100, 1331–1337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2009). Biological and environmental initial conditions shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first years of life. Developmental Science, 12(1), 194–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported in part by grants awarded to JFK Partners, University of Colorado School of Medicine from the Administration on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, University Center of Excellence in Developmental Disabilities Grant 90DD0699, and the Maternal Child Health Bureau, Leadership Education in Neurodevelopmental Disabilities (LEND) Grant T73MC11044 .

Author Contributions

Beth M. McManus: Dr. McManus conceptualized the study, conducted analyses, and wrote and revised manuscript drafts. Steven Rosenberg: Dr. Rosenberg conceptualized the study and wrote and revised manuscript drafts. Cordelia Robinson: Dr. Robison oversaw the analytic approach and reviewed and revised manuscript drafts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beth M. McManus.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose, financial or otherwise. All authors listed on the manuscript have seen and approved the submission of this version of the manuscript and take full responsibility for the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McManus, B.M., Robinson, C.C. & Rosenberg, S.A. Identifying Infants and Toddlers at High Risk for Persistent Delays. Matern Child Health J 20, 639–645 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-015-1863-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-015-1863-2

Keywords

Navigation