Skip to main content
Log in

The Influence of Accounts and Remorse on Mock Jurors’ Judgments of Offenders

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

Defendants often provide accounts that minimize their responsibility for the accused offense. Jurors attribute responsibility to defendants and decide legal outcomes based on the given account. The current research examined the effects of accounts (i.e., excuse, justification, denial, and no explanation) and the defendant’s remorse display (i.e., remorseful, remorseless) on mock jurors’ judgments. Participants acquitted the defendant in the denial condition most often and recommended the most lenient punishment in the justification condition. The remorseful defendant was found guilty more frequently than the remorseless defendant in the no explanation and (marginally) excuse conditions. Limitations and future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. While a true control condition (i.e., no account) would be ideal, it would not be realistic to have a defendant who testified but gave no account whatsoever. However, to come as close to a “no account” control condition as possible, we created a condition in which the defendant says he did not kill the victim but offers no explanation.

  2. We also ran a series of ordinal regression analyses, but, because the most extreme verdict was rarely chosen, we were forced to collapse the “second degree murder” and “voluntary manslaughter” categories to be able to model interaction effects. Results from these analyses on a three-category ordinal variable were essentially the same as the ones obtained with logical regression for a dichotomous guilty–not guilty variable.

  3. The believability data summarized in Table 3 refer to the perceived believability of the defendant. Note that this variable is distinct from the previously analyzed perceived believability of the defendant’s account, though the two variables are correlated, r = .68, p < .001.

References

  • Akehurst, L., Köhnken, G., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (1996). Lay persons’ and police officers’ beliefs regarding deceptive behaviour. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 461–471. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199612)10:6<461::AID-ACP413>3.0.CO;2-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, M., & Dewberry, C. (1994). “I’ve said I’m sorry, haven’t I?” A study of the identity implications and current restraints that apologies create for their recipients. Current Psychology (New Brunswick, NJ), 13, 10–21. doi:10.1007/BF02686855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, W. L. (1995). Accounts, excuses, & apologies: A theory of image restoration strategies. Albany, NY: State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, B. H. (1999). The ecological validity of jury simulations: Is the jury still out? Law and Human Behavior, 23, 75–91. doi:10.1023/A:1022326807441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darby, B. W., & Schlenker, B. R. (1989). Children’s reaction to transgressions: Effects of the actor’s apology, reputation, and remorse. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 353–364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, N., Park, J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Effect of blameworthiness and outcome severity on attributions of responsibility and damage awards in comparative negligence cases. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 597–617. doi:10.1023/A:1024856613829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukuno, M., & Ohbuchi, K. (1998). How effective are different accounts of harm-doing in softening victim’s reactions? A scenario investigation of the effects of severity, relationship, and culture. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 167–178. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, S. P. (1998). Aggravation and mitigation in capital cases: What do jurors think? Columbia Law Review, 98, 1538–1576. doi:10.2307/1123305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gold, G., & Weiner, B. (2000). Remorse, confession, group identity and expectancies about repeating a transgression. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 291–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzales, M. H., Haugen, J. A., & Manning, D. J. (1994). Victims as “narrative critics”: Factors influencing rejoinders and evaluative responses to offenders’ accounts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 691–704. doi:10.1177/0146167294206007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, C. L. (1987). A comparison of accounts: When is a failure not a failure? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 6, 117–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, W. P., Stone, J., Darley, J. M., & Grannemann, B. D. (2003). Yes, I did it, but don’t blame me: Perceptions of excuse defenses. The Journal of Psychiatry & Law, 31, 187–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, S. K., & Berger, C. R. (1974). Communication and justice: Defendant attributes and their effects on the severity of his sentence. Speech Monographs, 41, 282–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinke, C. L., Wallis, R., & Stalder, K. (1992). Evaluation of a rapist as a function of expressed intent and remorse. The Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 525–537.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazare, A. (2004). On apology. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedermeier, K. E., Horowitz, I. A., & Kerr, N. L. (2001). Exceptions to the rule: The effects of remorse, status, and gender on decision making. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 604–623. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02058.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer, J. E., Brigham, J. C., & Robinson, T. (1996). Euthanasia on trial: Examining public attitudes toward nonphysician-assisted death. The Journal of Social Issues, 52, 119–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pipes, R. B., & Alessi, M. (1999). Remorse and a previously punished offense in assignment of punishment and estimated likelihood of a repeated offense. Psychological Reports, 85, 246–248. doi:10.2466/PR0.85.5.246-248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pogrebin, M., Stretesky, P. B., Unnithan, N. P., & Venor, G. (2006). Retrospective accounts of violent events by gun offenders. Deviant Behavior, 27, 479–501. doi:10.1080/01639620600721429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proeve, M. J., & Howells, K. (2006). Effects of remorse and shame and criminal justice experience on judgments about a sex offender. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 145–161. doi:10.1080/10683160512331316271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. T., Smith-Lovin, L., & Tsoudis, O. (1994). Heinous crime or unfortunate accident? The effects of remorse on responses to mock criminal confessions. Social Forces, 73, 175–190. doi:10.2307/2579922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, M. G. (1976). Effects of defendant background and remorse on sentencing judgment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 6, 64–68. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1976.tb01312.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scher, S. J., & Darley, J. M. (1997). How effective are the things people say to apologize? Effects of the realization of the apology speech act. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 127–140. doi:10.1023/A:1025068306386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R., & Darby, B. W. (1981). The use of apologies in social predicaments. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 271–278. doi:10.2307/3033840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schönbach, P. (1980). A category system for account phases. European Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 195–200. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420100206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, M. B., & Lyman, S. M. (1968). Accounts. American Sociological Review, 33, 46–62. doi:10.2307/2092239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, M. A. (2004). Born again on death row: Retribution, remorse, and religion. The Catholic Lawyer, 43, 311–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2007). Moderators of nonverbal indicators of deception: A meta-analytic synthesis. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 13, 1–34. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.13.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundby, S. E. (1998). The capital jury and absolution: The intersection of trial strategy, remorse, and the death penalty. Cornell Law Review, 83, 1557–1598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C., & Kleinke, C. L. (1992). Effects of severity of accident, history of drunk driving, intent, and remorse on judgments of a drunk driver. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1641–1655. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00966.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, R. L., & Rinehart, N. (1986). Psychological causality in the attribution of responsibility for rape. Sex Roles, 14, 369–382. doi:10.1007/BF00288422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alayna Jehle.

About this article

Cite this article

Jehle, A., Miller, M.K. & Kemmelmeier, M. The Influence of Accounts and Remorse on Mock Jurors’ Judgments of Offenders. Law Hum Behav 33, 393–404 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9164-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9164-6

Keywords

Navigation