Skip to main content
Log in

Mugshot Exposure Effects: Retroactive Interference, Mugshot Commitment, Source Confusion, and Unconscious Transference

  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

More than 25 years of research has accumulated concerning the possible biasing effects of mugshot exposure to eyewitnesses. Two separate metaanalyses were conducted on 32 independent tests of the hypothesis that prior mugshot exposure decreases witness accuracy at a subsequent lineup. Mugshot exposure both significantly decreased proportion correct and increased the false alarm rate, the effect being greater on false alarms. A mugshot commitment effect, arising from the identification of someone in a mugshot, was a substantial moderator of both these effects. Simple retroactive interference, where the target person is not included among mugshots and no one in a mugshot is present in the subsequent lineup, did not significantly impair target identification. A third metaanalysis was conducted on 19 independent tests of the hypothesis that failure of memory for facial source or context results in transference errors. The effect size was more than twice as large for “transference” studies involving mugshot exposure in proximate temporal context with the target than for “bystander” studies with no subsequent mugshot exposure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Experimental psychologists have used the term interference to refer to the tendency for one activity to negatively impact memory for another activity. Retroactive interference would reflect a negative impact on memory for material encoded prior to the activity in question.

  2. Deffenbacher et al. (1981) have referred to memory for the target face image(s) per se as item memory and memory for the source or context of encounter with a face as context memory.

  3. Even though the cutoff year for the aforementioned comprehensive metaanalysis project was 2002, the Perfect and Harris (2003) article was included for the limited purpose of conducting the present metaanalysis focused just on mugshot exposure effects. This study is one of three published since 2000 and adds six independent tests to a prior total of 26 tests of the hypothesis that mugshot exposure is detrimental to eyewitness memory accuracy. The initial year for the current narrowly focused metaanalyses was determined by the fact that the first published study of mugshot exposure effects occurred in 1977.

  4. Inasmuch as the power to detect a difference between proportions is not simply a function of the magnitude of the difference, but varies as a function of where a given difference occurs along the scale of differences between zero and one, an arcsin transformation (nonlinear) of these differences has been suggested by Cohen (1988). The statistic h is the resulting transform of a given difference in proportions. For instance, the following pairs of proportions all yield approximate values of h =.20: .05 and .10, .20 and .29, .40 and .50, .60 and .70, .80 and .87, and .90 and .95.

REFERENCES

  • References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the metaanalyses.

  • Bartlett, J. C., & Fulton, A. (1991). Familiarity and recognition of faces: The factor of age. Memory & Cognition, 19, 229–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Brigham, J. C., & Cairns, D. (1988). The effect of mugshot inspections on eyewitness identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 1393–1410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Brown, E. L., Deffenbacher, K. A., & Sturgill, W. (1977). Memory for faces and the circumstances of encounter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 311–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S. E. (2005). A re-examination of the effects of biased lineup instructions in eyewitness identification. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 575–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Cutler, B. L., Penrod, S. D., & Martens, T. K. (1987). The reliability of eyewitness identification: The role of system and estimator variables. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 233–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Cutler, B. L., Penrod, S. D., & O'Rourke, T. E. (1986). Unconfounding the effects of contextual cues on eyewitness identification accuracy. Social Behaviour, 1, 113–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U. S. 579 (1993).

  • *Davies, G., Shepherd, J., & Ellis, H. (1979). Effects of interpolated mugshot exposure on accuracy of eyewitness identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 232–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K. (2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 687–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Deffenbacher, K. A., Carr, T. H., & Leu, J. R. (1981). Memory for words, pictures, and faces: Retroactive interference, forgetting, and reminiscence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 299–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Dysart, J. E., Lindsay, R. C. L., Hammond, R., & Dupuis, P. (2001). Mugshot exposure prior to lineup identification: Interference, transference, and commitment effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 1280–1284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Gorenstein, G. W., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1980). Effect of choosing an incorrect photograph on a later identification by an eyewitness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 616–622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C., Brown, J., & Jasechko, J. (1989). Becoming famous overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the past. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 326–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88, 67–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., Tubb, V. A., Hosch, H. M., & Memon, A. (2001). On the “general acceptance” of eyewitness testimony research: A new survey of the experts. American Psychologist, 56, 405–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Lindsay, R. C. L., Nosworthy, G. J., Martin, R., & Martynuck, C. (1994). Using mug shots to find suspects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 121–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Loftus, E. F. (1976). Unconscious transference in eyewitness identification. Law and Psychology Review, 2, 93–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Memon, A., Hope, L., Bartlett, J., & Bull, R. (2002). Eyewitness recognition errors: The effects of mugshot viewing and choosing in young and old adults. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1219–1227.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Perfect, T. J., & Harris, L. J. (2003). Adult age differences in unconscious transference: Source confusion or identity blending? Memory & Cognition, 31, 570–580.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Read, J. D., Tollestrup, P., Hammersley, R., McFadzen, E., & Christensen, A. (1990). The unconscious transference effect: Are innocent bystanders ever misidentified? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 3–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2006). The influence of emotion on memory in forensic settings. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross, & Lindsay, R. C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Memory for events (Vol. 1). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Ross, D. F., Ceci, S. J., Dunning, D., & Toglia, M. P. (1994). Unconscious transference and mistaken identity: When a witness misidentifies a familiar but innocent person. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 918–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, P. N., & Penrod, S. D. (1986). A meta-analysis of facial identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 139–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons v. United States, 390 U. S. 377 (1968).

  • Steblay, N. M. (1992). A meta-analytic review of the weapon focus effect. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 413–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. (1963). The proof of guilt. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise, R. A., & Safer, M. A. (2004). What U. S. judges know and believe about eyewitness testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 427–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kenneth A. Deffenbacher.

About this article

Cite this article

Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H. & Penrod, S.D. Mugshot Exposure Effects: Retroactive Interference, Mugshot Commitment, Source Confusion, and Unconscious Transference. Law Hum Behav 30, 287–307 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9008-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9008-1

Key words:

Navigation