Skip to main content
Log in

Eyewitness Identification Accuracy and Response Latency

  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Eyewitness identification research has reliably shown that accurate identifications are faster than inaccurate identifications. Recently, D. Dunning and S. Perretta (2002) claimed that an identification latency of 10–12 s not only best discriminates between accurate and inaccurate identifications but also produces extremely high accuracy rates, approaching 90%. Consistent with predictions from recognition memory theory, however, we show that the optimum time boundary varies with overall response latency under manipulations of retention interval and nominal lineup size, and that the accuracy rate inside the optimum time boundary is much less impressive than previously reported. We outline directions for clarifying the accuracy and latency relationship to assist the reliable diagnosis of identification accuracy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Values of Cohen's f greater than .4 are considered large effects, while the cut-off values for small and medium effects are .1 and .25, respectively.

  2. Time boundary analyses can also be conducted by computing the log odds ratio at each time boundary. As the log odds ratio analyses produce the same pattern of results as the chi square analyses, we report only the chi square analyses.

REFERENCES

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. Learning, memory and thinking (pp. 243–293). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N., Gordon, M., & Bond, N. (2000). Effect of photoarray exposure duration on eyewitness identification accuracy and processing strategy. Psychology, Crime & Law, 6, 21–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N., Weber, N., & Semmler, C. (2005). Eyewitness identification. In N. Brewer, & K. D. Williams (Eds.), Psychology and law: An empirical perspective (pp. 177–221). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N., Weber, N., & Semmler, C. (in press). A role for theory in eyewitness identification research. In R. C. L. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. Toglia (Eds.), Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Volume 2: Memory for people. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Perretta, S. (2002). Automaticity and eyewitness accuracy: A 10- to 12-second rule for distinguishing accurate from inaccurate positive identifications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 951–962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Stern, L. B. (1994). Distinguishing accurate from inaccurate eyewitness identifications via inquiries about decision processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 818–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahana, M., & Loftus, G. (1999). Response time versus accuracy in human memory. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of cognition (pp. 323–384). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., & Wells, G. L. (1980). What price justice? Exploring the relationship of lineup fairness to identification accuracy. Law and Human Behavior, 4, 303–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., & Wells, G. L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups: Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 556–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malpass, R. S., & Devine, P. G. (1981). Eyewitness identification: Lineup instructions and the absence of the offender. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 482–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (1996). Metacognitive processes. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory handbook of perception and cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 381–407). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Murdock, B. B. (1985). An analysis of the strength-latency relationship. Memory and Cognition, 13, 511–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., & Dufty, P. O. (1972). Strength theory and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 284–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., Lindsay, R. C. L., & Pryke, S. (2000). Postdictors of eyewitness errors: Can false identifications be diagnosed? Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 542–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., Lindsay, R. C. L., Pryke, S., & Dysart, J. E. (2001). Postdictors of eyewitness errors: Can false identifications be diagnosed in the cross-race situation? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S. L. (1992). Post-dicting eyewitness accuracy: Confidence, decision-times and person descriptions of choosers and nonchoosers. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 157–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S. L. (1993). Eyewitness identification accuracy, confidence, and decision times in simultaneous and sequential lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S. L. (1994). Decision times and eyewitness identification accuracy in simultaneous and sequential lineups. In D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments (pp. 300–327). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Zandt, T. (2000). ROC curves and confidence judgments in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 582–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, N., Brewer, N., Wells, G. L., Semmler, C., & Keast, A. (2004). Eyewitness identification and response latency: The unruly 10–12 second rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 139–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Leippe, M. R., & Ostrom, T. M. (1979). Guidelines for empirically assessing the fairness of a lineup. Law and Human Behavior, 3, 285–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by grants A00104516 and DP0556876 from the Australian Research Council.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Neil Brewer.

About this article

Cite this article

Brewer, N., Caon, A., Todd, C. et al. Eyewitness Identification Accuracy and Response Latency. Law Hum Behav 30, 31–50 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9002-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9002-7

KEY WORDS:

Navigation