Skip to main content
Log in

Self-Control Theory and Nonlinear Effects on Offending

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

This paper examines Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford, 1990) self-control theory and develops theoretical arguments for why self-control may have a differential effect on offending depending on the level of self-control.

Methods

We test the argument that the association between self-control and violent offending (n = 5,681) and non-violent offending (5,672) is nonlinear by using generalized propensity score analyses of data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health.

Results

The results indicate that self-control and offending are nonlinearly related in a manner that involves two thresholds. Specifically, among individuals at the high end of the self-control spectrum, there was little evidence of an association between variation in self-control and offending. However, among individuals in the middle part of the self-control spectrum, a positive association obtained—that is, the greater the level of low self-control, the greater the likelihood of offending. Finally, among individuals at the low end of the self-control spectrum, there was, once again, little evidence of an association.

Conclusions

A nonlinear association between self-control and offending may exist and have implications for self-control theory and tests of it. Studies are needed to investigate further the possibility of a nonlinear association and to test empirically the mechanisms that give rise to it.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Hirschi (2004: 548) has stated that “[a] change in the conceptualization of the sources of self-control and the cognitive processes it involves should have little effect on the empirical predictions derived from the theory.” Similarly, Piquero and Bouffard (2007: 7) have emphasized that the restatement does not fundamentally “change the general theory of crime.”

  2. Accordingly, Hirschi and Gottfredson (2008: 220), commenting on the suggestion that in some studies (e.g., Grasmick et al. 1993) opportunity measures outperform self-control measures and that self-control theory is thereby weakened, have observed: “Per contra, we would say, these findings show that the measure of opportunity is the better measure of control.”

  3. Some scholars have suggested that Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) treat self-control as a dichotomy—one is either low in self-control or high in self-control (Tittle 1995)—but their description of self-control clearly suggests a concept that is continuous (Goode 2008: 13), which is how it is almost invariably measured in empirical research.

  4. Agnew (2005: 136) has argued that “while plausible, I tend to reject this [ceiling effect] argument based on the assumption that few people reach such extreme positions.” In reality, ceiling effects, as with “tipping point” effects, simply require, as a general matter, that some threshold be reached. The threshold does not have to be extreme.

  5. An extensive literature on self-control exists in psychology. Within this literature, there is not a universally agreed upon definition or measurement of self-control. Even so, Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) discussion of self-control and their reconceputalization (Hirschi 2004; see also Hirschi and Gottfredson 2008) accords with many of the descriptions of self-control prevalent within this field (see, e.g., Hassin et al. 2010; Vohs and Baumeister 2011).

  6. There is no negative binomial option within Stata’s generalized propensity score procedure. To check that the results would be unlikely to be affected by use of the truncated measure, we compared results from regression models that used the truncated and untruncated measures, respectively, with those from negative binomial models using the truncated and untruncated measures, respectively. No appreciable difference in the results surfaced.

  7. Model fit was assessed using R2 (.069 for non-violent offending and .027 for violent offending). We compared specifications that included polynomials for self-control and the generalized propensity score and interactions between self-control and the generalized propensity score. The model in the paper was both the most parsimonious and had the best fit.

  8. We conducted ancillary analyses using a traditional regression modeling approach, including self-control and control variables and polynomial terms for self-control. When these results were graphed, they largely mirrored the generalized propensity score results shown here. We proceed with the latter modeling approach because it more directly addresses and highlights potential imbalances in covariates and makes no assumption about the functional form of the self-control and offending relationship (Hirano and Imbens 2004). In our study, the use of dichotomous or count measures of offending do not impose nonlinearity precisely because of this feature of the generalized propensity score approach (see, e.g., Doyle 2011; Kluve et al. 2012).

  9. The prediction model for self-control is provided in “Appendix 2”. As noted above, the model accords with that found in other studies of self-control using the same data. To conserve space, therefore, our discussion centers around the generalized propensity score results.

  10. The protocol outlined in discussions of generalized propensity score analyses involve “blocking on the score,” that is, dividing the treatment measure at two cut points to create three groups (see, e.g., Hirano and Imbens 2004; Bia and Mattei 2008; Kluve et al. 2012). We experimented with other cut-offs, creating, for example, five groups. The balance on covariates was largely the same regardless, suggesting that the balance was not sensitive to these cut points.

  11. The generalized propensity score procedure allows for specifying ordinal logistic regression models. We compared the results presented here with those using ordinal regression. The same pattern surfaced. However, with the latter approach, separate figures are generated for each level of the outcome (i.e., 0, 1, 2, 3, 4+). Given the similar results, we proceeded with the ordinary least squares estimates because they are simpler to present and discuss.

  12. It bears noting that, as Kluve et al. (2012: 19), echoing Hirano and Imbens (2004), have noted, “whether all the estimated coefficients associated with the [generalized propensity score] terms are equal to 0 can indicate whether the covariates introduce any bias.” If there are statistically significant coefficients involving the generalized propensity score, that indicates that the generalized propensity score is helpful in removing potential bias that results from the covariates. In Table 3, the statistically significant coefficients involving the generalized propensity score thus can be viewed as evidence that the methodology helps to reduce covariate-related bias in the estimation of the effect of self-control on offending.

  13. Full descriptions of the generalized propensity score modeling steps and procedures are detailed elsewhere. Hirano and Imbens (2004) provide proofs for their extension of the propensity score matching methodology to continuous treatments. Bia and Mattei (2008) describe the methodology and its implementation within Stata. Other works (e.g., Kluve et al. 2012) describe the methodology in greater detail and describe the advantages of generalized propensity score matching as compared to conventional regression-based approaches to estimating treatment effects. The advantages parallel those identified in using propensity score matching to estimate binary treatment effects (Rosenbaum and Rubin 1983).

  14. We conducted ancillary analyses in which we focused only on individuals who reported engaging in a non-violent offense. The argument here is that perhaps unique selection effects exist among those who commit non-violent offenses as compared to those who commit no such offenses. The same basic, nonlinear pattern emerged; the results are available upon request.

  15. We used the empirical Bayes fitted value as an estimate of offending propensity for each individual. This variable had a mean of 0 and ranged from −1.03 to 5.10. The reliability score for the overall offending intercept in our HLM model was .63. This score is slightly lower than those reported by Osgood and Schreck (2007) but is higher than ones reported in other studies using the same IRT approach (see, e.g., Sullivan et al. 2009; McGloin et al. 2011).

  16. We thank one of the reviewers for suggesting these additional lines of investigation.

  17. With generalized propensity score analyses, one does not specify separate functional forms to test which best predicts the outcomes. Rather, the focus is on achieving balance on the covariates and then, conditional on the generalized propensity score, generating predicted values. The resulting predicted values conformed with what was anticipated with hypothesis 3.

  18. A reviewer suggested analyzing multiple data sets to verify the results reported here. We agree, as noted in the text, that replication of this study is needed, with replication focusing on these data but also on the wide range of other data sets that exist on self-control and offending (Pratt and Cullen 2000; Goode 2008). In our view, the importance of other researchers replicating the findings here, and doing so with a wide range of other data sets and analytic approaches, is essential. On occasions when researchers report results from multiple studies, we may have greater confidence in the results. At the same time, the risk exists that the different studies a researcher selects are not representative of what would emerge from analyses of the larger universe of data sets. A central justification of meta-analyses, which have become more common in the past two decades, is precisely that they provide a more objective foundation on which to base particular claims. More generally, scientific progress results when independent researchers examine the same data and other data sets and arrive at similar results or determine the conditions under which results may vary; doing so reduces, but does not eliminate, the risk of confirmation bias (see Ioannidis 2005; Littell 2008; Eisner 2009). When a large number of data sets exist for studying a phenomenon, as is the case with self-control theory (see, e.g., Pratt and Cullen 2000; Goode 2008), this approach is especially important to ensure that findings are consistent across a range of researchers, measures, populations, and analytic approaches, and, conversely, to avoid situations whereby researchers unintentionally select data, measures, model specifications, and analytic approaches that favor one finding over another.

References

  • Abbott A (1988) Transcending general linear reality. Sociol Theory 6:169–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Agnew R (2005) Why do criminals offend?. Roxbury, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Apel RJ, Sweeten G (2010) Propensity score matching in criminology and criminal justice. In: Piquero AR, Weisburd D (eds) The handbook of quantitative criminology. Springer, New York, pp 543–562

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM (2007) The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:351–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM (2008) Nonshared environmental influences on adolescent delinquent involvement and adult criminal behavior. Criminology 46:341–370

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Mears DP, Stewart EA (2009) Low self-control and contact with the criminal justice system in a nationally representative sample of males. Justice Q 26:695–715

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG, Wright JP, Boutwell BB (2008) The relationship between self-control and language: evidence of a shared etiological pathway. Criminology 46:939–970

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Ferguson CJ, Whaley JL (2010) The association between parenting and levels of self-control: a genetically informative analysis. Crim Justice Behav 37:1045–1065

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker S, Ichino A (2002) Estimation of average treatment effects based on propensity score. Stata J 2:358–377

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk RA (2004) Regression analysis: a constructive critique. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Bia M, Mattei A (2008) A Stata package for the estimation of the dose–response function through adjustment for the generalized propensity score. Stata J 8:354–373

    Google Scholar 

  • Blalock HM (1967) Toward a theory of minority-group relations. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Breen R (2009) Formal theory in the social sciences. In: Hedström P, Wittrock B (eds) Frontiers of sociology. Brill, The Netherlands, pp 209–230

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brenkus J (2010) The perfection point: sport science predicts the fastest man, the highest jump, and the limits of athletic performance. Harper, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Brezina T, Tekin E, Topalli V (2009) A multimethods approach to anticipated early death and youth crime. Criminology 47:1091–1129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt CL, Gottfredson MR (eds) (2003) Control theories of crime and delinquency. Transaction, New Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapple CL, Vaske J, Hope TL (2010) Sex differences in the causes of self-control: an examination of mediation, moderation, and gendered etiologies. J Crim Justice 38:1122–1131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cota-Robles S, Neiss M, Rowe DC (2002) The role of puberty in violent and nonviolent delinquency among Anglo American, Mexican American, and African American boys. J Adolesc Res 17:364–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crosnoe R, Elder GH Jr (2004) Family dynamics, supportive relationships, and educational resilience during adolescence. J Fam Issues 25:571–602

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiPrete TA, Gangl M (2004) Assessing bias in the estimation of causal effects: Rosenbaum bounds on matching estimators and instrumental variables estimation with imperfect instruments. Sociol Methodol 34:271–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle WR (2011) Effects of increased academic momentum on transfer rates: an application of the generalized propensity score. Econ Educ Rev 30:191–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisner M (2009) No effects in independent prevention trials: can we reject the cynical view? J Exp Criminol 5:163–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ertefaie A, Stephens DA (2010) Comparing approaches to causal inference for longitudinal data: inverse probability weighting versus propensity scores. Int J Biostat 6:(2):1–22, article 14

    Google Scholar 

  • Felson RB, Osgood DW (2008) Violent crime. In: Goode E (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford, pp 160–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Freese L (1980) Formal theorizing. Annu Rev Sociol 6:187–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrett WE, Kirkendall DT (eds) (2000) Exercise and sport science. Lippincott, Philadelphia

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs JP (1972) Sociological theory construction. Dryden Press, Hinsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs JP (1997) Seven dimensions of the predictive power of sociological theories. Natl J Sociol 11:1–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson CL, Sullivan CJ, Jones S, Piquero AR (2010) ‘Does it take a village?’ Assessing neighborhood influences on children’s self-control. J Res Crime Delinq 47:31–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goode E (ed) (2008) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR (2006) The empirical status of control theory in criminology. In: Cullen FT, Wright JP, Blevins KR (eds) Taking stock: the status of criminological theory. Transaction, New Brunswick, pp 77–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (2003) Self-control and opportunity. In: Britt CL, Gottfredson MR (eds) Control theories of crime and delinquency. Transaction, New Brunswick, pp 5–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ Jr, Arneklev BJ (1993) Testing the core empirical implications of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 30:5–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg DF (1994) The historical variability of the age–crime relationship. J Quant Criminol 10:361–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guion RM (1998) Some virtues of dissatisfaction in the science and practice of personnel selection. Hum Resour Manag Rev 8:351–365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo S, Fraser MW (2010) Propensity score analysis: statistical methods and applications. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Hage J (ed) (1994) Formal theory in sociology: opportunity or pitfall?. State University of New York Press, Albany

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris KM, Florey F, Tabor J, Bearman PS, J Jones, Udry JR (2003) The national longitudinal study of adolescent health: research design. Available on-line: www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design

  • Harte J (2007) Human population as a dynamic factor in environmental degradation. Popul Environ 28:223–236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassin RR, Ochsner KN, Trope Y (eds) (2010) Self-control in society, mind, and brain. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay C, Forrest W (2006) The development of self-control: examining self-control theory’s stability thesis. Criminology 44:739–774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay C, Forrest W (2008) Self-control and the concept of opportunity: the case for a more systematic union. Criminology 46:1039–1072

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynie DL (2001) Delinquent peers revisited: does network structure matter? Am J Sociol 106:1013–1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirano K, Imbens GW (2004) The propensity score with continuous treatments. In: Gelman A, Meng X-L (eds) Applied Bayesian modeling and causal inference from incomplete data perspectives. Wiley, New York, pp 73–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T (2004) Self-control and crime. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD (eds) Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications. Guilford Press, New York, pp 537–552

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR (2000) In defense of self-control. Theor Criminol 41:55–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR (2008) Critiquing the critics: the authors respond. In: Goode E (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford, pp 217–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Homans GC (1967) The nature of social science. Harcourt, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidis JPA (2005) Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med 2:e124 (online). doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124

  • Jasso G (1988) Principles of theoretical analysis. Sociol Theory 6:1–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluve J, Schneider H, Uhlendorff A, Zhao Z (2012) Evaluating continuous training programmes by using the generalized propensity score. J R Stat Soc 175:1–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Littell JH (2008) Evidence-based or biased? The quality of published reviews of evidence-based practices. Child Youth Serv Rev 30:1299–1317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Londeree BR (1997) Effect of training on lactate/ventilatory thresholds: a meta-analysis. Med Sci Sports Exerc 29:837–843

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord RG, Diefendorff JM, Schmidt AM, Hall RJ (2010) Self-regulation at work. Annu Rev Psychol 61:543–568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loughran TA, Mulvey EP, Schubert CA, Fagan J, Piquero AR, Losoya SH (2009) Estimating a dose–response relationship between length of stay and future recidivism in serious juvenile offenders. Criminology 47:699–740

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magen E, Gross JJ (2010) Getting our act together: toward a general model of self-control. In: Hassin RR, Ochsner KN, Trope Y (eds) Self-control in society, mind, and brain. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 335–353

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus B (2004) Self-control in the general theory of crime: theoretical implications of a measurement problem. Theor Criminol 8:33–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Schreck CJ, Stewart EA, Ousey GC (2011) Predicting the violent offender: the discriminant validity of the subculture of violence. Criminology 49:767–794

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead NL, Alquist JL, Baumeister RF (2010) Ego depletion and the limited resource model of self-control. In: Hassin RR, Ochsner KN, Trope Y (eds) Self-control in society, mind, and brain. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 375–388

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Merton RK (1945) Sociological theory. Am J Sociol 50:462–473

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt TE, Arsenault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington HL, Houts R, Poulton R, Roberts BW, Ross S, Sears MR, Thomson WM, Caspi A (2011) A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci (early edition 1/24/11):1–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108

  • Muraven M, Pogarsky G, Shmueli D (2006) Self-control depletion and the general theory of crime. J Quant Criminol 22:263–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood DW, Schreck CJ (2007) A new method for studying the extent, stability, and predictors of individual specialization in violence. Criminology 45:273–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ousey GC, Wilcox P (2007) The interaction of antisocial propensity and life-course varying predictors of delinquent behavior: differences by method of estimation and implications for theory. Criminology 45:313–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrone D, Sullivan CJ, Pratt TC, Margaryan S (2004) Parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 48:298–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR (2009) Self-control theory. In: Krohn M, Lizotte A, Hall G (eds) Handbook on crime and deviance. Springer, New York, pp 153–168

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Bouffard J (2007) Something old, something new: a preliminary investigation of Hirschi’s redefined self-control. Justice Q 24:1–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Jennings WG, Farrington DP (2010) On the malleability of self-control: theoretical and policy implications regarding a general theory of crime. Justice Q 27:803–834

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pogarsky G (2010) Deterrence and decision making: research questions and theoretical refinements. In: Krohn M, Lizotte A, Hall G (eds) Handbook on crime and deviance. Springer, New York, pp 241–258

    Google Scholar 

  • Pong S-l, Johnston J, Chen V (2010) Authoritarian parenting and asian adolescent school performance: insights from the U.S. and Taiwan. Int J Behav Dev 34:62–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT (2000) The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:931–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhomberg LR, Goodman JE, Haber LT, Dourson M, Andersen ME, Klaunig JE, Meek B, Price PS, McClellan RO, Cohen SM (2011) Linear low-dose extrapolation for noncancer health effects is the exception, not the rule. Crit Rev Toxicol 41:1–19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB (1983) The central role of the propensity score in observation studies for causal effects. Biometrika 70:41–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sampson RJ (2009) Racial stratification and the durable tangle of neighborhood inequality. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci 621:260–280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreck CJ, Fisher BS, Mitchell Miller J (2004) The social context of violent victimization: a study of the delinquent peer effect. Justice Q 21:23–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz S (2006) Beyond self-control: analysis and critique of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. Duncker and Humbolt, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Seipel C, Eifler S (2010) Opportunities, rational choice, and self-control: on the interaction of person and situation in a general theory of crime. Crime Delinq 56:167–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sellers CS (1999) Self-control and intimate violence: an examination of the scope and specification of the general theory of crime. Criminology 37:375–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson SS, Geis G (2008) The underdeveloped concept of opportunity. In: Goode EA (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford, pp 49–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorensen A (2009) Statistical models and mechanisms of social processes. In: Hedström P, Wittrock B (eds) Frontiers of sociology. Brill, The Netherlands, pp 369–400

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart EA (2010) Matching methods for causal inference: a review and a look forward. Stat Sci 25:1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stults BJ, Baumer EP (2007) Racial context and police force size: evaluating the empirical validity of the minority threat perspective. Am J Sociol 113:507–546

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan CJ, McGloin JM, Ray JV, Caudy MS (2009) Detecting specialization in offending: comparing analytic approaches. J Quant Criminol 25:419–441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweeten G (2012) Scaling criminal offending. J Quant Criminol 28:533–557

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR (1995) Control balance: towards a general theory of deviance. Westview, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR (2011) Self-control theory and the management of violence. In: Heitmeyer W, Haupt H-G, Kirschner A, Malthaner S (eds) The control of violence. Springer, New York, pp 91–120

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Udry JR (2003) The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health). Waves I and II, 1994–1996 [machine-readable data file and documentation]. University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Carolina Population Center, Chapel Hill

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn MG, DeLisi M, Beaver KM, Wright JP (2009) Identifying latent classes of behavioral risk based on early childhood manifestations of self-control. Youth Violence Juv Justice 7:16–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vohs KD, Baumeister RF (eds) (2011) Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (2002) Companions in crime: the social aspects of criminal conduct. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Whitmeyer J (2009) The perils of assuming a single equilibrium. Sociol Perspect 52:557–579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winship C, Morgan SL (1999) The estimation of causal effects from observational data. Annu Rev Sociol 25:659–706

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winslow EB, Shaw DS (2007) Impact of neighborhood disadvantage on overt behavior problems during early childhood. Aggress Behav 33:207–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright BRE, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA (2001) The effects of social ties on crime vary by criminal propensity: a life-course model of interdependence. Criminology 39:321–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu J, Liska AE (1993) The certainty of punishment: a reference group effect and its functional form. Criminology 31:447–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman GM, Messner SF (2011) Neighborhood context and nonlinear peer effects on adolescent violent crime. Criminology 49:873–904

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Avi Bhati, Michela Bia, Sam Field, Alessandra Mattei, Sonja Siennick, and Brian Stults, and especially the editors and anonymous reviewers, for their helpful suggestions and guidance. This research uses data from Add Health, a program project designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris, and funded by a grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 17 other agencies. Special acknowledgment is due to Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Persons interested in obtaining data files from Add Health should contact Add Health, Carolina Population Center, 123 W. Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27516-2524 (addhealth@unc.edu). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis. We thank Avi Bhati, Michela Bia, Sam Field, Alessandra Mattei, Sonja Siennick, and Brian Stults, and especially the editors and anonymous reviewers, for their helpful suggestions and guidance

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel P. Mears.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 4.

Table 4 Low self-control scale items

Appendix 2

See Table 5.

Table 5 Generalized propensity score predictive model for low self-control

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mears, D.P., Cochran, J.C. & Beaver, K.M. Self-Control Theory and Nonlinear Effects on Offending. J Quant Criminol 29, 447–476 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-012-9187-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-012-9187-5

Keyword

Navigation