Skip to main content
Log in

How Do They ‘End Up Together’? A Social Network Analysis of Self-Control, Homophily, and Adolescent Relationships

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Self-control theory (Gottfredson and Hirschi 1990) argues that individuals with similar attributes tend to ‘end up together’ (i.e., homophily) because of the tendency to select friends based on self-control. Studies documenting homophily in peer groups interpret the correlation between self-control, peer delinquency, and self-reported delinquency as evidence that self-control is an influential factor in friendship formation. However, past studies are limited because they do not directly test the hypothesis that self-control influences friendship selection, nor do they account for other mechanisms that may influence decisions. As a result, it is unclear whether the correlation between individual and peer behavior is the result of selection based on self-control or alternative mechanisms. To address this gap in the literature this study employs exponential random graph modeling to test hypotheses derived from self-control theory using approximately 63,000 respondents from 59 schools from the National Longitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health (Add Health). In contrast to the predictions made by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990), and the conclusions drawn from prior research, there is little evidence that self-control influences friendship selection. The findings are embedded in past work on the relationship between self-control and peer relationships, and implications for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Though not a common nomenclature in the criminological literature, selective mixing characterizes the process of friendship formation implicit in the social causation model of crime and delinquency (see Wright et al. 1999).

  2. Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) also argue that the relationship between peer and individual behavior can partially be explained as an artifact of self-report measures. Research supporting this argument is discussed briefly below, but not in detail here as this is a separate mechanism generating the relationship between peer and individual behavior (i.e., measurement error).

  3. A separate question, which is often found in research on mediating effects, focuses on whether there is a moderating relationship between self-control and peer delinquency on self-reported delinquency. These studies investigate whether the effect of self-control on delinquency depends on peer delinquency or whether the effect of peer delinquency on delinquency depends on self-control, often referred to as social amplification effects (Wright et al. 2001). Unlike research on mediating effects, research on social amplification is less consistent. Several studies find social amplification effects (Gibson and Wright 2001; Wright et al. 2001; Ousey and Wilcox 2007, cf. Doherty 2006), while others, using more accurate measures of peer delinquency, have found opposite (Meldrum et al. 2009) or null effects (McGloin and Shermer 2009). These mixed findings indicate that the moderating effects of self-control and peer delinquency on delinquency are not well understood.

  4. Preliminary analyses included a variable recording the race/ethnicity of the respondent since race/ethnicity has been shown to be an important predictor of friendship formation (see Moody 1999). This variable was created for non-Hispanic white, African-American, Hispanic, Native American and Asian respondents. Since fewer than half of the 59 schools contained sufficient variation in race for analysis, it was not possible to estimate the effects of race across all the schools. Using the schools with sufficient variability, models including dummy variables for race produce results that are substantively similar to the results presented below. They are excluded here due to space constraints. Full models are available by request from the author.

  5. Students who were not nominated by respondents or who were not included in the sample due to absence on the days in which the survey was conducted create a separate problem. Unless these cases are missing completely at random (Rubin, 1976), inference about the network may be biased due to a non-random mechanism as they are unobserved in the observed data matrix, s. Schools with the highest response rates were included in this analysis due to this problem. Note, however, that this situation is not unique to relational data. Inference about the social network may be biased, but so will inference about individual covariates under a separate modeling framework. As with any study dealing with missing data, assumptions are made to facilitate statistical inference when the problem cannot be addressed by the model. The assumption made in this model is that these cases are missing completely at random.

  6. The coefficients are the Markov chain Monte Carlo estimates. Post estimation inspection of the chains show sufficient mixing and convergence. All non-zero coefficients are significantly different from zero at the 0.001 level.

  7. The visual difference in the plots for the observed network between Figs. 2 and 5 is caused by differences in the limits on the y-axis for each plot. The same network is analyzed and displayed in each figure.

  8. The complete saturated sample contains 3,702 respondents in 16 schools. However, one school containing 44 respondents is excluded from the analysis due to insufficient network data for conducting meaningful analyses. This school does not appear to be significantly different from the schools analyzed. Compared to the saturated sample, the other schools in the Wave I in-home survey used a different question to measure peer data: respondents could nominate 1 male and 1 female friend. The sample of the social network in each school is substantially limited as a result.

  9. The influence of school characteristics requires an estimation of a hierarchical model testing the presence of a random effect for self-control and exploring cross-level interactions. Unfortunately, hierarchical models for ERG models are not available. Running models for each school and then investigating the dispersion of the effects of self-control (the approach used here) offers a crude means of approximating the distribution for the self-control parameter. However, the finding that there is little variability in the effect across schools suggests that this approximation is not entirely inadequate for the problem at hand.

References

  • Arneklev BJ, Grasmick HJ, Bursik RJ Jr (1999) Evaluating the dimensionality and invariance of ‘low self-control’. J Quant Criminol 15:307–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron SW (2004) Self-control, social consequences, and criminal behavior: street youth and the general theory of crime. J Res Crim Delinq 40:403–425

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bearman P, Moody J, Stovel K (2004) Chains of affection: the structure of adolescent romantic and sexual networks. Am J Sociol 110:44–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG (2008) Genetic influences on the stability of low self-control: results from a longitudinal sample of twins. J Crim Justice 36:478–485

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Mears PD, Stewart E (2009) Low self-control and contact with the criminal justice system in a nationally representative sample of males. Justice Q 26:695–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite J (1989) Crime, shame, and reintegration. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne D (1971) The attraction paradigm. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapple CL (2005) Self-control, peer relations, and delinquency. Justice Q 22:89–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doherty EE (2006) Self-control, social bonds, and desistance: a test of life-course interdependence. Criminology 44:807–833

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans TD, Cullen FT, Burton VS Jr, Dunaway RG, Benson M (1997) The social consequences of self-control: testing the general theory of crime. Criminology 35:475–504

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank O, Strauss D (1986) Markov graphs. J Am Stat Assoc 81:832–842

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geyer CJ, Thompson EA (1992) Constrained Monte Carlo maximum likelihood for dependent data. J R Stat Soc Series B 54:657–699

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson C, Wright J (2001) Low self-control and coworker delinquency: a research note. J Crim Justice 29:483–492

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson CL, Wright JP, Tibbetts SG (2000) An empirical assessment of the generality of the general theory of crime: the effects of low self-control on social development. J Crim Justice 23:109–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Gile K, Handcock MS (2006) Model-based assessment of the impact of missing data on inferences for networks. Center for statistics and the social sciences working paper no. 66

  • Glueck S, Glueck E (1950) Unraveling juvenile delinquency, Commonwealth Fund, New York

  • Goodreau SM, Kitts JA, Morris M (2009) Birds of a feather, or friend of a friend?: using exponential random graph models to investigate adolescent social networks. Demography 46:103–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ, Arneklev B (1993) Testing the core empirical implications Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. J Res Crim Delinq 30:5–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagan J, McCarthy B (1998) Mean streets: youth crime and homelessness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Handcock MS (2003) Assessing degeneracy in statistical models of social networks. Center for statistics and the social sciences working paper no. 39

  • Handcock MS, Gile K (2007) Modeling social networks with sampled or missing data. Center for statistics and the social sciences working paper no. 75

  • Handcock MS, Gile K (2010) Modeling networks from sampled data. Annals of Applied Statistics forthcoming

  • Handcock MS, Hunter DR, Butts CT, Morris M (2008) Ergm: a package to fit, simulate and diagnose exponential-family models for networks. J Stat Soft 24:1–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay C (2001) Parent, self-control, and delinquency: a test of self-control theory. Criminology 39:707–736

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynie D (2001) Delinquent peers revisited: does network structure matter? Am J Sociol 106:1013–1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynie D (2002) Friendship networks and delinquency: the relative nature of peer delinquency. J Quant Criminol 18:99–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynie D, Osgood W (2005) Reconsidering peers and delinquency: how do peers matter? Soc Forces 84:1109–1130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland PW, Leinhardt S (1981) An exponential family of probability distributions for directed graphs. J Am Stat Assoc 76:33–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houtzager B, Baerveldt C (1999) Just like normal: a social network study of the relation between petty crime and the intimacy of adolescent friendships. Soc Behav Pers 27:177–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter DR (2007) Curved exponential family models for social networks. Soc Networks 29:216–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter DR, Handcock MS (2006) Inference in curved exponential family models for networks. J Comput Graph Stat 15:565–583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter DR, Goodreau SM, Handcock MS (2008) Goodness of fit of social network models. J Am Stat Assoc 103:248–258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koehly LM, Goodreau SM, Morris M (2004) Exponential family models for sampled and census network data. Sociol Methodol 34:241–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kossinets G, Watts DJ (2009) Origins of homophily in an evolving social network. Am J Sociol 115:405–450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarsfeld P, Merton RK (1954) Friendship as a social process: a substantive and methodological analysis. In: Berger M, Abel T, Page CH (eds) Freedom and control in modern society. Van Nostrand, New York, pp 18–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Longshore D, Turner S, Stein JA (1996) Self-control in a criminal sample: an examination of construct validity. Criminology 34:209–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longshore D, Turner S, Stein JA (1998) Reliability and validity of a self-control measure: rejoinder. Criminology 36:175–182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longshore D, Chang E, Hsieh S, Messina N (2004) Self-control and social bonds: a combined control perspective on deviance. Crim Delinq 50:542–564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Piquero AR (2010) On the relationship between co-offending, network redundancy and offending versatility. J Res Crim Delinq 47:63–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Shermer LO (2009) Self-control and deviant peer network structure. J Res Crim Delinq 46:35–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Pratt TC, Maahs J (2004) Rethinking the IQ-delinquency relationships: a longitudinal analysis of multiple theoretical models. Justice Q 21:603–635

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Sullivan CJ, Piquero AR (2009) Aggregating to versatility?: transitions among offender types in the short term. Br J Criminol 49:243–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPherson JM, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM (2001) Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Ann Rev Sociol 27:415–444

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meldrum RC, Young JTN, Weerman FM (2009) Reconsidering the effect of self-control and delinquent peers: implications of measurement for theoretical significance. J Res Crim Delinq 46:353–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moody J (1999) The structure of adolescent social relations: modeling friendship in dynamic social settings. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC

  • Nagin DS, Pogarsky G (2004) Time and punishment: delayed consequences and criminal behavior. J Quant Criminol 20:295–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nooney JG (2005) Religion, stress, and mental health in adolescence: findings from add health. Rev Relig Res 46:341–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ousey GC, Wilcox P (2007) The interaction of antisocial propensity and life-course varying predictors of delinquent behavior: differences by method of estimation and implications for theory. Criminology 45:313–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parra GR, DuBois DL, Sher KJ (2006) Investigation of profiles of risk factors for adolescent psychopathology: a person-centered approach. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 35:386–402

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster R, Pogarsky G (2009) Rational choice, agency, and thoughtful reflective decision making: the short and long-term consequences of making good choices. J Quant Criminol 25:103–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrone D, Sullivan CJ, Pratt TC, Margaryan S (2004) Parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 48:298–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Rosay AB (1998) The reliability and validity of Grasmick et al.’s self-control scale: a comment on Longshore et al. Criminology 36:157–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, MacIntosh R, Hickman M (2000) Does self-control affect survey response? Applying exploratory, confirmatory and item response theory analysis to Grasmick et al’.s self-control scale. Criminology 38:897–930

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT (2000) The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:931–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins G, Pattison P, Kalish Y, Lusher D (2007) An introduction to exponential random graph (p*) models for social networks. Soc Networks 29:173–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin DB (1976) Inference and missing data. Biometrika 63:581–592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreck CJ, Stewart EA, Fisher BS (2006) Self-control, victimization, and their influence on risky lifestyles: a longitudinal analysis using panel data. J Quant Criminol 22:319–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snijders TAB (2002) Markov chain Monte Carlo estimation of exponential random graph models. J Social Struct 3:1–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Snijders TAB, Baerveldt C (2003) A multilevel network study of the effects of delinquent behavior on friendship evolution. J Math Sociol 27:123–151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snijders TAB, Pattison PE, Robins GL, Handcock MS (2006) New specifications for exponential random graph models. Sociol Methodol 36:99–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (1996) Organization and instigation in delinquent groups. Criminology 34:11–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (2002) Companions in crime. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (2007) The tangled web: delinquency, deception, and parental attachment. J Youth Adolesc 36:607–622

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman S, Pattison P (1996) Logit models and logistic regressions for social networks: I. An introduction to markov graphs and p*. Psychometrika 60:401–425

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weerman FM, Smeenk WH (2005) Peer similarity in delinquency for different types of friends: a comparison using two different measurement methods. Criminology 43:499–523

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weis J, Hawkins DJ (1981) Preventing delinquency. OJJDP, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright B, Entner R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA (1999) Low self-control, social bonds, and crime: social causation, social selection, or both? Criminology 37:479–514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright BRE, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PE (2001) The effects of social ties on crime vary by criminal propensity: a life-course model of interdependence. Criminology 39:321–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jacob T. N. Young.

Appendix A

Appendix A

See Table 2.

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for variables by school (N = 59)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Young, J.T.N. How Do They ‘End Up Together’? A Social Network Analysis of Self-Control, Homophily, and Adolescent Relationships. J Quant Criminol 27, 251–273 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-010-9105-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-010-9105-7

Keywords

Navigation