Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Use of a Reading Lexicon to Aid Contextual Vocabulary Acquisition by EFL Arab Learners

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article investigates how the use of a deliberate approach of analyzing a given reading may predict differences in CVA effectiveness. Sixty Arab EFL learners were randomly assigned to an experimental group and a control group, thirty participants for each. The experimental group received training in the deliberate Clarke and Nation (System 8:211–220, 1980) CVA technique, whereas the control group were not guided through a training method. Then, both groups were asked to answer three vocabulary tests and then participate to finish a series of six readings adjusted using Nation’s K-level reading lexicon to control the difficult words during readings. After treatments, the students took a post-test vocabulary session. Results show that the group that have used the deliberate CVA technique retained about twice as many new words as the other group did. That is the use of a deliberate-CVA methodology significantly improves learning. The experimental manipulation produced a learning effect that was 76.1% greater than that of the control group in terms of word context recognition and 128.0% greater than that of the control group in terms of word definition accuracy. Pedagogical implications, limitations and directions for further research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alkhawaldeh, A. (2012). High school students’ challenges in English reading comprehension in Amman Second Directorate of Education. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 39, 214–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, W. S. (1966). The development of a classification scheme of contextual aids. Reading Research Quarterly, 2, 57–82. https://doi.org/10.2307/747039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnaud, P. J. L., & Béjoint, H. (1992). Vocabulary and applied linguistics. Basingstoke, UK: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1978). The role of dialogue in language acquisition. In A. Sinclair, R. J. Jarvelle, & W. J. M. Levelt (Eds.), The child’s concept of language. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, R., & McCarthy, M. (1988). Vocabulary and language teaching. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Child, D. (2016). Readability score. [Computer program.] Bolney, Sussex, UK: Added Bytes Ltd. Retrieved from https://readability-score.com/text.

  • Clarke, D. F., & Nation, I. S. P. (1980). Guessing the meanings of words from context. System, 8, 211–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/0346-251X(80)90003-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, T. (2016). Compleat lexical tutor v.8 [Computer program.] Montréal, QC: Université du Québec à Montréal. Retrieved from http://www.lextutor.ca/vp/comp.

  • Cremer, M. (2013). Accessing word meaning: Semantic word knowledge and reading comprehension in Dutch monolingual and bilingual fifth-graders. (Doctoral dissertation). University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands.

  • Elgort, I. (2011). Deliberate learning and vocabulary acquisition in a second language. Language Learning: A Journal of Research in Language Studies, 61, 367–413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00613.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, B. K. (2012). Relationships between vocabulary size, working memory, and phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21, 109–123. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0063).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hafner, L. E., Weaver, W. W., & Powell, K. (1970). Psychological and perceptual correlates of reading achievement among fourth graders. Journal of Reading Behavior, 2, 281–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862967009546909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon, J. M., Wood, K. D., & Kiser, K. (2009). Promoting vocabulary learning with interactive word wall. Middle School Journal, 40(3), 58–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, L., Olson, A., & Humphreys, G. (2013). Overcoming the effect of letter confusability in letter-by-letter reading: A rehabilitation study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 23, 429–462. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2013.776500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horst, M., Cobb, T., & Meara, P. (1998). Beyond A Clockwork Orange: Acquiring second language vocabulary through reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 11, 207–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kibby, M. W. (1995). The organization and teaching of things and the words that signify them. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 39, 208–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirmizi, Ö., & Topcu, N. (2014). Vocabulary learning strategies of Turkish EFL students at Karabük University. Journal of Graduate School of Social Sciences, 18, 217–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Frazier Norbury, C. (2015). Making inferences from text: It’s vocabulary that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1224–1232. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madrigal-Hopes, D. L., Villavicencio, E., Foote, M. M., & Green, C. (2014). Transforming English language learners’ work readiness: Case studies in explicit, work-specific vocabulary instruction. Adult Learning, 25(2), 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/1045159514522432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meara, P. (1997). Models of vocabulary acquisition. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 109–121). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morett, L. (2019). The power of an image: Images, not glosses, enhance learning of concrete L2 words in beginning learners. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9623-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueser, A. M. (1984). Practicing vocabulary in context: Teacher’s guide. New York: Macmillan/McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueser, A. M., & Mueser, J. A. (1984). Practicing vocabulary in context. New York: SRA/McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. Boston, MA: Heinle&Heinle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pimsleur, P. (1967). A memory schedule. Modern Language Journal, 51, 73–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1967.tb06700.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitts, M., White, H., & Krashen, S. (1989). Acquiring second language vocabulary through reading: A replication of the Clockwork Orange study using second language acquirers. Reading in a Foreign Language, 5, 271–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, W. J. (2003).”What Is the ‘Context’ for Contextual Vocabulary Acquisition?”. In P.P. Slezak (Ed.) Proceedings of the 4th Joint International Conference on Cognitive Science/7th Australasian Society for Cognitive Science Conference (ICCS/ASCS-2003; Sydney, Australia) (Sydney: University of New South Wales) (Vol. 2, pp. 547–552).

  • Rapaport, W. J., & Kibby, M. W. (2014). Contextual vocabulary acquisition: From algorithm to curriculum. In A. Palma (Ed.). Castañeda and his guises: Essays on the work of Hector-Neri Castañeda (pp. 107–150). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Powell, J. S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878–893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.38.8.878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Powell, J. S., & Kaye, D. B. (1983). Teaching vocabulary-building skills: A contextual approach. In A. C. Wilkinson (Ed.), Classroom computers and cognitive science (pp. 121–143). New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–398. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Daalen-Kapteijns, M. M., & Elshout-Mohr, M. (1981). The acquisition of word meanings as a cognitive learning process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 386–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. J., & Wood, C. (2012). Sensitivity to the acoustic correlates of lexical stress and their relationship to reading in skilled readers. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 267–280. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0122-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, J. P. (2009). An experimental study on the effects of different reading tasks on L2 vocabulary acquisition. English Language Teaching, 2(3), 69–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Deanship of Scientific Research at Najran University for funding this project (NU/SHED/16/065).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mohammed Ali Mohsen.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Script for Initial Training, Experimental Group

EFL Paragraph Exercises

For Learning the Deliberate Contextual Vocabulary Acquisition (CVA) Technique

DAY ONE

“Please read the first paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for the word “cad” (difficulty level 7).

  1. 1.

    What part of speech is it? [noun, singular, concrete]

  2. 2.

    Look at the whole clause. What is the logic? [description of a person]

  3. 3.

    Look at the whole sentence and surrounding sentences. Do you see any other clues?

[opposite of hero; different writers may disagree a lot]

  1. 4.

    Now give a meaning to the word. [a man without honor]

The trouble with history is that it can be used to prove whatever you want it to prove. One historian turns to one set of facts and proves one thing. Another prefers another set of facts or perhaps reads the same set in a different way and proves something else. Some historians treat a man like a hero and others make him out to be a cad. As long as personal preference rules the day in the selection of historical data, all conclusions are suspect and probably misleading.

“Now read the second paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for the word “empowered” (difficulty level 5).

  1. 1.

    What part of speech is it? [adjective, past participle]

  2. 2.

    Look at the whole clause. What is the logic? [someone who can do great things]

  3. 3.

    Look at the whole sentence and surrounding sentences. Do you see any other clues?

[implies self-confidence and self-trust]

  1. 4.

    Now give a meaning to the word. [free to make one’s own decisions]

Walter Lippmann once observed that religious experience gives people a new confidence in their power and potential as human beings. The feeling that one stands in the immediate presence of God is associated with inner assurance, inner discipline, inner strength. The problems of this world lose their importance, and a person begins for the first time to feel the full significance of individuality. A new self-confidence and self-trust show themselves in every area, including the economic, and the empowered individual begins to do great things with his life.

Appendix 2: Script for Initial Training, Experimental Group

DAY TWO

“Please read the first paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for the word “bewildered” (difficulty level 5).

  1. 1.

    What part of speech is it? [adjective, past participle]

  2. 2.

    Look at the whole clause. What is the logic? [expecting to experience something]

  3. 3.

    Look at the whole sentence and surrounding sentences. Do you see any other clues?

[people did not expect to understand Emerson’s lectures]

  1. 4.

    Now give a meaning to the word. [very deeply confused]

Ralph Waldo Emerson was born in Boston. The son of a minister, he tried at first to follow his father’s example. After a few years he threw off the cloth and went out on the lecture circuit, where he soon became a star attraction. When he came to Pittsburgh in 1851, businesses closed early so that young office workers could go listen to him. Many were expecting to find themselves bewildered: it had been said of his philosophical works that his appeal arose from the fact, not that people understood him, but that they believed minds such as his should be encouraged. But when he began, the confusion cleared, and those who attended the lectures reported that what he said was just common sense.

“Now read the second paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for the word “entrepreneurs” (difficulty level 4).

  1. 1.

    What part of speech is it? [noun, plural, concrete]

  2. 2.

    Look at the whole clause. What is the logic? [other people]

  3. 3.

    Look at the whole sentence and surrounding sentences. Do you see any other clues?

[people like Honda and Morita, but smaller, hence with smaller businesses]

  1. 4.

    Now give a meaning to the word. [people who start new businesses]

This is what happened in Japan. We all know about the great institutions created by men of exceptional talent, Soichiro Honda, for example, or Sony’s Akio Morita. The achievements of such men were an essential part of the Japanese success story. But they were not the whole story. Important also were smaller entrepreneurs, who created tiny enterprises to supply the industrial giants with high-quality parts. Important also were millions of workers who were excited about their jobs and committed to the production of high-quality cars. (A story from the early eighties tells about a worker who stopped to straighten the wind-shield wipers of every Honda that he passed on his way home from work: he just couldn’t stand to see an imperfection in a Honda.)

Appendix 3: Script for Initial Training, Control Group

EFL Paragraph Exercises

For the Control Group

DAY ONE

“Please read the first paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for a word that you do not understand. [cad, hero, historian, misleading, preference]

  1. 1.

    (Call on a student.) What word did you identify?

  2. 2.

    (To everyone.) Who knows what the word means?

  3. 3.

    (To the first student.) Does the meaning work in this sentence?

(To everyone.) What other meanings are possible?

  1. 4.

    (Call on another student, and repeat the process.)

The trouble with history is that it can be used to prove whatever you want it to prove. One historian turns to one set of facts and proves one thing. Another prefers another set of facts or perhaps reads the same set in a different way and proves something else. Some historians treat a man like a hero and others make him out to be a cad. As long as personal preference rules the day in the selection of historical data, all conclusions are suspect and probably misleading.

“Now read the second paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for a word that you do not understand. [assurance, empowered, individuality, potential]

  1. 1.

    (Call on a student.) What word did you identify?

  2. 2.

    (To everyone.) Who knows what the word means?

  3. 3.

    (To the first student.) Does the meaning work in this sentence?

(To everyone.) What other meanings are possible?

  1. 4.

    (Call on another student, and repeat the process.)

Walter Lippmann once observed that religious experience gives people a new confidence in their power and potential as human beings. The feeling that one stands in the immediate presence of God is associated with inner assurance, inner discipline, inner strength. The problems of this world lose their importance, and a person begins for the first time to feel the full significance of individuality. A new self-confidence and self-trust show themselves in every area, including the economic, and the empowered individual begins to do great things with his life.

Appendix 4: Script for Initial Training, Control Group

DAY TWO

“Please read the first paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for a word that you do not understand. [appeal, bewildered, circuit, encouraged, lecture]

  1. 1.

    (Call on a student.) What word did you identify?

  2. 2.

    (To everyone.) Who knows what the word means?

  3. 3.

    (To the first student.) Does the meaning work in this sentence?

(To everyone.) What other meanings are possible?

  1. 4.

    (Call on another student, and repeat the process.)

Ralph Waldo Emerson was born in Boston. The son of a minister, he tried at first to follow his father’s example. After a few years he threw off the cloth and went out on the lecture circuit, where he soon became a star attraction. When he came to Pittsburgh in 1851, businesses closed early so that young office workers could go listen to him. Many were expecting to find themselves bewildered: it had been said of his philosophical works that his appeal arose from the fact, not that people understood him, but that they believed minds such as his should be encouraged. But when he began, the confusion cleared, and those who attended the lectures reported that what he said was just common sense.

“Now read the second paragraph on your sheet.” (Students read.)

Look for a word that you do not understand. [entrepreneurs, essential, imperfection]

  1. 1.

    (Call on a student.) What word did you identify?

  2. 2.

    (To everyone.) Who knows what the word means?

  3. 3.

    (To the first student.) Does the meaning work in this sentence?

(To everyone.) What other meanings are possible?

  1. 4.

    (Call on another student, and repeat the process.)

This is what happened in Japan. We all know about the great institutions created by men of exceptional talent, Soichiro Honda, for example, or Sony’s Akio Morita. The achievements of such men were an essential part of the Japanese success story. But they were not the whole story. Important also were smaller entrepreneurs, who created tiny enterprises to supply the industrial giants with high-quality parts. Important also were millions of workers who were excited about their jobs and committed to the production of high-quality cars. (A story from the early eighties tells about a worker who stopped to straighten the wind-shield wipers of every Honda that he passed on his way home from work: he just couldn’t stand to see an imperfection in a Honda.)

Appendix 5: Technical Attributes of the Selected Readings

Note: Calculations are from Child (2016), except for new-word data (Cobb 2016).

Reading 1: “The Values of an Achieving Society,” by Harold B. Jones, Jr.

Source: Jones, H. B. Jr. (2002). Personal character and national destiny (p. 13). St. Louis, MO: Paragon House.

FKRE

71.4

FKGL

5.4

GFC

8.7

CLI

10.8

SMOG

9.4

ARI

4.2

Spache

3.4

DCS

3.8

NRT

1:57

Sentim

2

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

4

Average new-word K-level:

4.50

Reading 2: “Some Lessons Learned by Seventy,” by Zell Miller.

Source: Miller, Z. (2003). A national party no more: The conscience of a conservative democrat (p. 189). Atlanta, GA: Stroud and Hall.

FKRE

86.5

FKGL

3.0

GFC

5.8

CLI

8.1

SMOG

7.4

ARI

1.3

Spache

2.7

DCS

2.0

NRT

2:01

Sentim

1

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

5

Average new-word K-level:

5.20

Reading 3: “Why Am I a Slave?” by Frederick Douglass.

Source: Douglass, F. (1855). My bondage and my freedom (p. 89). New York, NY: Dover, 1969.

FKRE

85.2

FKGL

3.6

GFC

7.2

CLI

8.2

SMOG

8.4

ARI

2.3

Spache

3.1

DCS

2.6

NRT

1:59

Sentim

1

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

6

Average new-word K-level:

5.50

Reading 4: “The Meaning of Money,” by Ayn Rand.

Source: Rand, A. (1957). Atlas shrugged (p. 410). New York, NY: Dutton, 2005.

FKRE

84.6

FKGL

3.6

GFC

6.1

CLI

7.7

SMOG

7.6

ARI

1.7

Spache

3.2

DCS

2.8

NRT

2:05

Sentim

1

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

7

Average new-word K-level:

5.50

Reading 5: “American Identity,” by Samuel P. Huntington.

Source: Huntington, S. P. (2004). Who are we? The challenges to America’s national identity (p. 8). New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

FKRE

47.7

FKGL

8.5

GFC

11.7

CLI

13.8

SMOG

10.8

ARI

6.4

Spache

6.4

DCS

5.8

NRT

1:44

Sentim

1

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

8

Average new-word K-level:

5.75

Reading 6: “We Have Earned the Whirlwind,” by Roy Moore.

Source: Moore, R. (2005). So help me God: The ten commandments, judicial tyranny, and the battle for religious freedom (p. 143). Nashville, TN: Broadman and Holman.

FKRE

69.8

FKGL

6.2

GFC

8.7

CLI

11.1

SMOG

9.5

ARI

5.5

Spache

4.2

DCS

4.5

NRT

1:50

Sentim

3

New-word count (difficulty level of K-4 or higher):

9

Average new-word K-level:

5.78

———

Key: FKRE (Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease); FKGL (Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level); GFC (Gunning-Fog Score); CLI (Coleman-Liau Index); SMOG (SMOG Index); ARI (Automatic Readability Index); Spache (Spache Score); DCS (Dall-Chall Score); NRT (normal reading time for native speakers); Sentim (sentiment index; see below).

Sentiment: 1 = neutral (slightly positive); 2 = neutral (moderately positive); 3 = positive.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Alyami, M., Mohsen, M.A. The Use of a Reading Lexicon to Aid Contextual Vocabulary Acquisition by EFL Arab Learners. J Psycholinguist Res 48, 1005–1023 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09644-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09644-z

Keywords

Navigation