Skip to main content
Log in

Vocal Cues Underlying Youth and Adult Portrayals of Socio-emotional Expressions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite known differences in the acoustic properties of children’s and adults’ voices, no work to date has examined the vocal cues associated with emotional prosody in youth. The current study investigated whether child (n = 24, 17 female, aged 9–15) and adult (n = 30, 15 female, aged 18–63) actors differed in the vocal cues underlying their portrayals of basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness) and social expressions (meanness, friendliness). We also compared the acoustic characteristics of meanness and friendliness to comparable basic emotions. The pattern of distinctions between expressions varied as a function of age for voice quality and mean pitch. Specifically, adults’ portrayals of the various expressions were more distinct in mean pitch than children’s, whereas children’s representations differed more in voice quality than adults’. Given the importance of pitch variables for the interpretation of a speaker’s intended emotion, expressions generated by adults may thus be easier for listeners to decode than those of children. Moreover, the vocal cues associated with the social expressions of meanness and friendliness were distinct from those of basic emotions like anger and happiness respectively. Overall, our findings highlight marked differences in the ways in which adults and children convey socio-emotional expressions vocally, and expand our understanding of the communication of paralanguage in social contexts. Implications for the literature on emotion recognition are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allgood, R., & Heaton, P. (2015). Developmental change and cross-domain links in vocal and musical emotion recognition performance in childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 398–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38, 1145–1160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bänziger, T., Hosoya, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2015). Path models of vocal emotion communication. PLoS ONE, 10, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayes, M. A. (1972). Behavioral cues of interpersonal warmth. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 39, 333–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaauw, E. (1994). The contribution of prosodic boundary markers to the perceptual difference between read and spontaneous speech. Speech Communication, 14, 359–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2015). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.4.09, retrieved 1 June 2015 from http://www.praat.org/.

  • Brosgole, L., & Weisman, J. (1995). Mood recognition across the ages. International Journal of Neuroscience, 82, 169–189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2008). The sound of sarcasm. Speech Communication, 50, 366–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chronaki, G., Hadwin, J. A., Garner, M., Maurage, P., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2014). The development of emotion recognition from facial expressions and non-linguistic vocalizations during childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R. (1988). Does happiness mean friendliness? Induced mood and heterosexual self-disclosure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 283–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Krom, G. (1995). Some spectral correlates of pathological breathy and rough voice quality for different types of vowel fragments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 794–811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ancoli, S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1125–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N., Harizuka, S., & Kumar, S. (2002). Cross-cultural patterns in emotion recognition: Highlighting design and analytical techniques. Emotion, 2, 75–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eskenazi, L., Childers, D. G., & Hicks, D. M. (1990). Acoustic characteristics of vocal quality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 33, 298–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eskritt, M., & Lee, K. (2003). Do actions speak louder than words? Preschool children’s use of the verbal-nonverbal consistency principle during inconsistent communications. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 25–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fant, G., Kruckenberg, A., & Nord, L. (1991). Prosodic and segmental speaker variations. Speech Communication, 10, 521–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, C. T. (2000). Harmonics-to-noise ratios in normally speaking prepubescent girls and boys. Journal of Voice, 14, 17–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, C. T. (2002). Harmonics-to-noise ratio: An index of vocal aging. Journal of Voice, 16, 480–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., & Giedd, J. (1999). Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: A study using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 1511–1522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fives, C. J., Kong, G., Fuller, J. R., & DiGiuseppe, R. (2011). Anger, aggression, and irrational beliefs in adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friend, M. (2000). Developmental changes in sensitivity to vocal paralanguage. Developmental Science, 3, 148–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobl, C., & Ní Chasaide, A. (2000). Testing affective correlates of voice quality through analysis and resynthesis. In ISCA tutorial and research workshop (ITRW) on Speech and Emotion.

  • Gorham-Rowan, M. M., & Laures-Gore, J. (2006). Acoustic-perceptual correlates of voice quality in elderly men and women. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 171–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Götestam Skorpen, C., & Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology and Aging, 12, 590–599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammerschmidt, K., & Jürgens, U. (2007). Acoustical correlates of affective prosody. Journal of Voice, 21, 531–540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harries, M. L. L., Walker, J. M., Williams, D. M., Hawkins, S., & Hughes, I. A. (1997). Changes in the male voice at puberty. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 77, 445–447.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heffernan, K. (2004). Evidence from HNR that /s/ is a social marker of gender. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics, 23, 71–84.

  • Hollien, H., Green, R., & Massey, K. (1994). Longitudinal research on adolescent voice change in males. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96, 2646–2654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hortensius, R., Schutter, D. J. L. G., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). When anger leads to aggression: Induction of relative left frontal cortical activity with transcranial direct current stimulation increases the anger-aggression relationship. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 342–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, P., & Kadi-Hanifi, K. (1991). Comparison of prosodic properties between read and spontaneous speech material. Speech Communication, 10, 163–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., Dearing, K. F., et al. (2002). Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger: Relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Development, 73, 1101–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iriondo, I., Guaus, R., Rodriguez, A., Lazaro, P., Montoya, N., Blanco, J. M., Bernadas, D., Oliver, J. M., Tena, D., & Longhi, L. (2000). Validation of an acoustical modeling of emotional expression in Spanish using speech synthesis techniques. In Proceedings of the ISCA ITRW on speech and emotion, Newcastle, 57 September, 2000 (pp. 161–166). Belfast: Textflow.

  • Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behaviour and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), The handbook of emotion (pp. 220–235). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jürgens, R., Grass, A., Drolet, M., & Fischer, J. (2015). Effect of acting experience on emotion expression and recognition in voice: Non-actors provide better stimuli than expected. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 39, 195–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2001). Impact of intended emotion intensity on cue utilization and decoding accuracy in vocal expression of emotion. Emotion, 1, 381–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., & Ekman, P. (2000). Facial expression of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 236–249). New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L. C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87, 820–857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. (2002). Smile boycotts and other body politics. Feminism & Psychology, 12, 319–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laukka, P., Linnman, C., Ahs, F., Pissiota, A., Frans, O., Faria, V., et al. (2008). In a nervous voice: Acoustic analysis and perception of anxiety in social phobics’ speech. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 32, 195–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leinonen, L., Hiltunen, T., Linnankoski, I., & Laakso, M. L. (1997). Expression of emotional-motivational connotations with a one-word utterance. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1853–1863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leppänen, J. M., & Hietanen, J. K. (2001). Emotion recognition and social adjustment in school-aged girls and boys. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 429–435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Development, 53, 991–1003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: Understanding the distress of peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 143–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maxim, L. A., & Nowicki, S. J. (2003). Developmental associations between nonverbal ability and social competence. Philosophy, Sociology and Psychology, 2, 745–758.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAllister, A., Sederholm, E., Sundberg, J., & Gramming, P. (1994). Relations between voice range profiles and physiological and perceptual voice characteristics in ten-year-old children. Journal of Voice, 8, 230–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAllister, A., Sundberg, J., & Hibi, S. R. (1996). Acoustic measurements and perceptual evaluation of hoarseness in children’s voices. Speech, Music and Hearing Quarterly Progress and Status Report, 4, 15–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. L. C., & Ross, E. D. (2013). Attitudinal prosody: What we know and directions for future study. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 471–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Mplus [Software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noble, L., & Xu, Y. (2011). Friendly speech and happy speech—are they the same? Proceedings of the 17th international congress of phonetic sciences. Hong Kong.

  • Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Gender differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer victimization: Social and physical aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 242–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pell, M. D., Paulmann, S., Dara, C., Alasseri, A., & Kotz, S. A. (2009). Factors in the recognition of vocally expressed emotions: A comparison of four languages. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 417–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 117–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pittam, J., & Scherer, K. R. (1993). Vocal expression and communication of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 185–197). New York, London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pons, F., & Harris, P. (2005). Longitudinal change and longitudinal stability of individual differences in children’s emotion understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1158–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reise, S. P., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Kim, K. H. (2005). An illustration of multilevel factor analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 126–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rochman, D., Diamond, G. M., & Amir, O. (2008). Unresolved anger and sadness: Identifying vocal acoustical correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, 505–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rockwell, P. (2000). Lower, slower, louder: Vocal cues of sarcasm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 483–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1989). Effect size estimation for one-sample multiple-choice-type data: Design, analysis, and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 332–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D. A., Panattoni, C., & Happé, F. (2012). Children’s recognition of emotions from vocal cues. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 97–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1996). Adding the affective dimension: A new look in speech analysis and synthesis. Proceedings of the 4th international conference of spoken language processing. Philadelphia, USA.

  • Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms. Speech Communication, 40, 227–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., Wallbott, H. G., & Goldbeck, T. (1991). Vocal cues in emotion encoding and decoding. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 123–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Koivumaki, J., & Rosenthal, R. (1972). Minimal cues in the vocal communication of affect: Judging emotions from content-masked speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1, 269–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Ladd, D. R., & Silverman, K. E. A. (1984). Vocal cues to speaker affect: Testing two models. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 1346–1356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., & Oshinsky, J. S. (1977). Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, R. (2002). Odd girl out: The hidden culture of aggression in girls. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spackman, M. P., Brown, B. L., & Otto, S. (2009). Do emotions have distinct vocal profiles? A study of idiographic patterns of expression. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1565–1588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stathopoulos, E. T., Huber, J. E., & Sussman, J. E. (2011). Changes in acoustic characteristics of the voice across the life span: Measures from individuals 4–93 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 1011–1021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stathopoulos, E. T., & Sapienza, C. (1993). Respiratory and laryngeal measures of children during vocal intensity variation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94, 2531–2543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, J. E., & Sapienza, C. (1994). Articulatory, developmental, and gender effects on measures of fundamental frequency and jitter. Journal of Voice, 8, 145–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toran, K. C., & Lal, B. K. (2009). Objective analysis of voice in normal young adults. Kathmandu University Medical Journal, 7, 374–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trentacosta, C. J., & Fine, S. E. (2010). Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 19, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, M. K. (2004). Glares of contempt, eye rolls of disgust and turning away to exclude: Non-verbal forms of social aggression among girls. Feminism Psychology, 14, 371–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Bezooijen, R., Otto, S. A., & Heenan, T. A. (1983). Recognition of vocal expressions of emotion: A three-nation study to identify universal characteristics. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 387–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ververidis, D., & Kotropoulos, C. (2006). Emotional speech recognition: Resources, features, and methods. Speech Communication, 48, 1162–1181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside, S. P., & Hodgson, C. (2000). Speech patterns of children and adults elicited via a picture-naming task: An acoustic study. Speech Communication, 32, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2008). The cognitive basis of trait anger and reactive aggression: An integrative analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 3–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yumoto, E., Gould, W. J., & Baer, T. (1982). Harmonics-to-noise ratio as an index of the degree of hoarseness. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71, 1544–1550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, S. (2008). Emotion recognition in Chinese natural speech by combining prosody and voice quality features. Advances in Neural Networks, 5264, 457–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zupan, B. (2015). Recognition of high and low intensity facial and vocal expressions of emotion by children and adults. Journal of Social Sciences and Humanities, 1, 332–344.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michele Morningstar.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the McGill Research Ethics Board, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 3.

Table 3 ANOVA on the effect of age, gender, expression, sentence, and take on vocal cues

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Morningstar, M., Dirks, M.A. & Huang, S. Vocal Cues Underlying Youth and Adult Portrayals of Socio-emotional Expressions. J Nonverbal Behav 41, 155–183 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0250-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0250-7

Keywords

Navigation