Skip to main content
Log in

The Interface Between Core Affects and the Challenge–Skill Relationship

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Different models have been developed to describe human experiences. One of these models is the core affect model, which states that the core of emotional experience is a simple state of feeling good or bad, energized or drowsy. Another model is the experience fluctuation model (EFM), which has been developed to map how emotions and other experiences fluctuate along with the relationship between challenges and skills. In this study, we first mathematically refined the EFM. Second, we tested if fluctuations in core affect could be modeled with a continuous wave-like sine function (Study 1). Third, we applied the sine function to our own experience-sampling data, which we collected via mobile phones from 55 university students, and located each challenge–skill relationship in a two-dimensional core affect space (Study 2). The results were consistent in both studies and showed that fluctuations in core affect dimensions, measured as a function of the relationship between challenges and skills, can be modeled with the sine function. The results also indicated that there is a systematic link between challenge–skill relationships and core affect: High challenge–high skill situations were connected to a very active and pleasant core affect, whereas high challenge–low skill situations were connected to a quite active but unpleasant core affect, low challenge–low skill situations to a very passive and unpleasant core affect, and low challenge–high skill situations to a quite passive and pleasant core affect. Compared to previous studies, this study presents a more fine-grained and comprehensive level of information on the relationship between core affect and challenges and skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barrett, L., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K., & Gross, J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, S., & Haworth, J. (1994). ‘Flow’ experience in the daily lives of sixth-form college students. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (1988). Optimal experience, psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: The use of experience sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 185–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of the Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 281–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Manzano, O., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullén, F. (2010). The psychophysiology of the flow during piano playing. Emotion, 10(3), 301–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., & Bassi, M. (2000). The quality of experience in adolescents’ daily lives: Developmental perspectives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126, 347–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2005). The investigation of optimal experience and apathy. European Psychologist, 4, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., Massimini, F., & Bassi, M. (2011). Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures. Social empowerment through personal growth. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, G., Voelkl, J., & Morris, C. (1994). Measurement and analysis issues with explanation of variance in daily experience using the flow model. Journal of Leisure Research, 26, 337–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E. L., Fredrickson, B., Kring, A., Johnson, D., Meyer, P., & Penn, D. (2010). Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 849–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hektner, J., Schmidt, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huta, V. (2012). Linking peoples’ pursuit of eudaimonia and hedonia with characteristics of their parents: Parenting styles, verbally endorsed values, and role modeling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 47–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimhy, D., Delespaul, P., & Corcoran, C. (2006). Computerized experience sampling method (ESMc). Assessing feasibility and validity among individuals with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 221–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Emotion (Vol. 13, pp. 25–59). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J., McGraw, A., & Cacioppo, J. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 684–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1988). The systematic assessment of flow in daily experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 266–287). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Carli, M. (1987). The monitoring of optimal experience: A tool for psychiatric rehabilitation. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 545–549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B. (2004). The flow model of intrinsic motivation in Chinese: Cultural and personal moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5(2), 181–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Models of concentration in natural environments: A comparative approach based on streams of experiential data. Social Behavior and Personality, 27, 603–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G., & Wong, Q. (2002). The daily affect of heroin addicts in a therapeutic comminity: A pilot study in Hong Kong. Social Behavior and Personality, 30, 561–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muukkonen, H., Hakkarainen, K., Jalonen, S., Kosonen, K., Heikkilä, A., Lonka, K., Inkinen, M., Salmela-Aro, K., Linnanen, J., & Salo, K. (2007). Process- and context-sensitive research on academic knowledge practices: Developing CASS-tools and methods. In Proceedings of the computer supported collaborative learning conference. New Jersey, USA: Rutgers University, July 16–21, 2007.

  • Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow theory and research. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 195–206). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, F., Berkhof, J., & Delespaul, P. (2006). Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion, 6, 383–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (2009). Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1259–1283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., & Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schallberger, U., & Pfister, R. (2001). Flow-erleben in arbeit und freizeit: Eine untersuchung zum ‘paradox der arbeit’ mit der experience sampling method (ESM). Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 45, 176–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, A., Shernoff, D. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Individual and situational factors related to the experience of flow in adolescence: A multilevel approach. In A. D. Ong & M. H. M. Dulmen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 542–558). New York, US: Oxford University Press.

  • Scollon, C., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling: Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 5–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 57–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirois, B., & Burg, M. (2003). Negative emotion and coronary heart disease: A review. Behavior Modification, 27, 83–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, Z., Foo, M.-D., & Uy, M. (2008). Mood spillover and crossover among dual-earner couples: A cell phone event sampling study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 443–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 65–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A., Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1999). On the dimensional and hierarchical structure of affect. Psychological Science, 10, 297–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and activation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolvanen, A., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., Hakkarainen, K., Inkinen, M., Lonka, K., et al. (2011). A new approach for estimating a nonlinear growth component in multilevel modelling. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 370–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yik, M., & Russell, J. (2004). On the relationship between circumplexes: Affect and Wiggins’ IAS. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 203–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yik, M., Russell, J., & Barrett, L. (1999). Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 600–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yik, M., Russell, J., & Steiger, J. (2011). A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect. Emotion, 11, 705–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Contributions were made by all researchers. The study was partially funded by the RYM Indoor Environment 462054 of the Finnish Funding Agency for Technology and Innovation, and Academy of Finland 134931, 139128 (PI Salmela-Aro) and 121207 (PI Kai Hakkarainen), and the Jacobs Foundation (PI Salmela-Aro). We would like to thank several persons who have contributed to researching and developing the CASS and our study, including Annamari Heikkilä, Satu Jalonen, Kari Kosonen, Lasse Lipponen, Juha Linnanen, Kristian London, Visa Parviainen, Niclas Sandström, and Petri Vesikivi.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mikko Inkinen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Inkinen, M., Lonka, K., Hakkarainen, K. et al. The Interface Between Core Affects and the Challenge–Skill Relationship. J Happiness Stud 15, 891–913 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9455-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9455-6

Keywords

Navigation