Skip to main content
Log in

Visualizing the Good Life: A Cross-Cultural Analysis

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How can we visualize similarity and difference across cultural conceptions of a good or worthy life? To examine the internal structure of such conceptions, we asked 584 university students from mainland China and Canada to indicate the relative importance of 30 elements or criteria commonly used in defining a good life. Statistical comparison and multidimensional scaling were used to reveal the form and extent of group commonalities and differences on the criteria and their meaning in relation to underlying dimensions of individual and cultural variation. The results suggested that South Asian Canadians were more oriented towards moral, spiritual, and beneficent concerns in envisioning a good life than were the mainland Chinese and Western European Canadians. The Chinese, for their part, tended to emphasize practical, prudential, and socially defined goods. This contrasted with the Western European Canadians, who showed more preference for personally defined, internal goods. East Asian Canadians fell between Chinese and Western European Canadians in overall orientation, reflecting their biculturality. All groups placed heavy emphasis on close and enduring relationships.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnett, J. J. (2002). The psychology of globalization. American Psychologist, 57, 774–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arzy, S., Collette, S., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., & Blanke, O. (2009). Subjective mental time line: The functional architecture of projecting the self to past and future. Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 2009–2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isolation and communion in western man. Boston: Beacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. (2000). The meme machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borg, I., & Groenen, P. J. F. (1997). Modern multidimensional scaling theory. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, I., & Groenen, P. J. F. (2005). Modern multidimensional scaling theory and applications. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. vol 3: Loss. New York: Basic Books.

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 21, 723–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busing, F. M. T. A., Groenen, P. J. F., & Heiser, W. J. (2005). Avoiding degeneracy in multidimensional unfolding by penalizing on the coefficient of variation. Psychometrika, 70, 49–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2008). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cozolino, L. (2006). The neuroscience of human relationships: Attachment and the developing social brain. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to account for cross-cultural response bias: A classification of score adjustment procedures and review of research in JCCP. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 263–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. (2007). Stumbling on happiness. London: Harpercollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold, T., Guthrie, D., & Wank, D. (2002). Social connections in China: Institutions, culture and the changing nature of guanxi. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gunde, R. (2002). Culture and customs of China. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption. New York: Touchstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R. (2006). Mapping global values. Comparative Sociology, 5, 115–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jahanbegloo, R. (2008). The spirit of India. New Delhi: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. (2003). The pursuit of happiness and the realization of sympathy: Cultural patterns of self, social relations, and well-being. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 113–161). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J. W., & Jones, P. S. (2002). Cultural differences in response to a Likert scale. Research in Nursing and Health, 25, 295–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lim, T. S., & Bowers, J. W. (1991). Facework: solidarity, approbation, and tact. Human Communication Research, 17, 415–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Main, M. (1999). Epilogue: Attachment theory: Eighteen points with suggestions for future studies. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory: Research and clinical applications (pp. 845–857). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Culture, self, and the reality of the social. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 277–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. J., Fung, H., & Mintz, J. (1996). Self-construction through narrative practices: A Chinese and American comparison of early socialization. Ethos, 24, 237–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, H. (1997). Ways of thinking of eastern peoples: India, China, Tibet, Japan revised (ed). London: Kegan Paul International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2002). Culture and cognition. In D. L. Medina (Ed.), Stevens handbook of experimental psychology. Wiley Online. doi: 10.1002/0471214426.

  • Palkivala, N. (2002). We, the people. New Delhi: UBS Publishers and Distributors.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London, B, Biological Sciences, 362, 773–786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, and applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1124, 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2006). A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications. Comparative Sociology, 5, 137–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture-specifics in the content and structure of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 92–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. P. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tafarodi, R. W., Bonn, G., Liang, H., Takai, J., Moriizumi, S., Belhekar, V., et al. (2012). What makes for a good life? A four-nation study. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 783–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tobin, J., Hsueh, Y., & Karasawa, M. (2009). Preschool in three cultures revisited: China, Japan, and the United States. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vyas, R. N. (1992). Nature of Indian culture. New Delhi: Concept Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (2002). The protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. P. Baehr & G. C. Wells, Trans. London: Penguin. (Original work published 1905).

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 131–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, X. (2000). An introduction to Confucianism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yeh, L. L., Kim, K. O., Chompreeda, P., Rimkeeree, H., Yau, N. J. N., & Lundahl, D. S. (1998). Comparison in use of the 9-point hedonic scale between Americans, Chinese, Koreans, and Thai. Food Quality and Preference, 9, 416–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by a Standard Research Grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (410–2006–1127) to the second author. We thank Lindie Liang, Sara Houshmand, Tiffany Jenzer, and Parisa Ghanbari for their assistance with data collection and translation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gregory Bonn.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bonn, G., Tafarodi, R.W. Visualizing the Good Life: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. J Happiness Stud 14, 1839–1856 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9412-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9412-9

Keywords

Navigation