Skip to main content
Log in

Prime and Performance: Can a CEO Motivate Employees Without Their Awareness?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Work motivation research is at a crossroads with the discovery of the causal effects of primed subconscious goals in addition to those of consciously set goals on performance. Although social psychologists continue to demonstrate positive effects of primed goals on a multitude of dependent variables, priming research has been criticized for its lack of generalizability beyond tightly controlled laboratory experiments. Addressing this skepticism, a field experiment was conducted in a for-profit organization, where the CEO used goal priming to motivate job performance. A performance goal for achievement was primed with achievement-related words embedded in an email from the CEO to employees. The goal priming by the CEO necessitated little to no costs yet it increased objectively measured performance effectiveness by 15% and efficiency by 35% over a 5-day work-week. This field experiment illustrates a new alternative for increasing employee performance. In a second experiment, we conducted a conceptual replication of the field experiment in the laboratory with a larger sample size, and we extended theory by testing a measure of motivation level as a mediator of the primed goal-performance effect. The results affirmed the hypothesized motivational influence. These two experiments increase understanding of subconscious motivation processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To ensure no other differences were infused between the two emails, we conducted Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count analysis (Pennebaker et al. 2015) to assess scores on achievement and several other dimensions between the prime and control group emails. Achievement scores between the conditions differed as hypothesized (prime = 12.12, control = 3.12), but positive emotion (prime = 9.09, control = 7.29), power (prime = 3.03, control = 3.12), reward (prime = 6.06, control, 4.17), future focus (prime = 1.01, control = 0), and tone (prime = 99, control = 99) did not differ between conditions. This suggests that we only primed an achievement goal.

  2. The difference in degrees of freedom occurred due to a printing error. We ran this experiment in several classes and one batch of protocols was printed without this item (it inadvertently went over to the next page when the file was opened in the copy center). We re-ran all analyses reported here and all the patterns of results remain the same.

References

  • Abelson, R. P. (1979). Differences between belief and knowledge systems. Cognitive Science, 3, 355–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1984). Automatic and conscious processing of social information. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 1–43). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and vognition (pp. 93–130). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. Handbook of social cognition, 1, pp. 1–40.

  • Bargh, J. A. (2005). Bypassing the will: Toward demystifying the nonconscious control of social behavior. The New Unconscious, 12, 37–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (2007). Social psychology and the unconscious. NY: Psychology Press.

  • Bargh, J. A. (2017). Before you know it: The unconscious reasons we do what we do. New York: Touchstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to primingand automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In W. D. Spaulding (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, vol. 41. Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion (pp. 71–124). Lincoln: U. of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 73–79.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2010). Motivation. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 268–316). NY: Wiley.

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caruso, E. M., Shapira, O., & Landy, J. F. (2017). Show me the money: A systematic exploration of manipulations, moderators, and mechanisms of priming effects. Psychological Science, 28, 1148–1159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Nonconscious motivations: Their activation, operation, and consequences. In A. Tesser, D. A. Stapel, & J. V. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation: Emerging psychological perspectives (pp. 13–41). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., & Latham, G. P. (2014). The effect of priming learning vs. performance goals on a complex task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125, 88–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Welcome back theory. Perspectives of Psychological Science, 9, 72–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., & Smith, P. K. (2005). The power of the subliminal: On subliminal persuasion and other potential applications. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman & J. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscisous (pp. 77–106). Oxford University Press.

  • Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L., & Aarts, H. (2007). Effects of priming and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious (pp. 51–131). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eden, D. (2017). Field experiments in organizations. Annual Review of OrganizationalPsychology and Organizational Behavior, 4, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganegoda, D. B., Latham, G. P., & Folger, R. (2016). The effect of a consciously set and a primed goal on fair behavior. Human Resource Management, 55, 789–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanratty, & Stahl (2015). 45% of executives cite time constraints as number-one problem in achieving professional goals, says new Economist Intelligence Unit report. The Economist, September 24.

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, A. (2013). The predictive value of conscious and sub-conscious motives on career advancement. In E. A. Locke & G. P. Latham (Eds.), New developments in goal setting and task performance (pp. 246–261). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2012). A proposal to deal with questions about priming effects. September 26.

  • Kruglanski, A., Shah, J., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chen, W., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 311–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Baldes, J. J. (1975). The “practical significance” of Locke’s theory of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 122–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Ernst, C. (2006). Keys to motivating tomorrow’s workforce. Human Resource Management Review, 16, 181–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B. (1974). Improving job performance through training in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 187–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (2018). Goal setting theory: Controversies and resolutions. In D. Ones, N. Anderson, C. Viswesvaran, & H. Sinangil (Eds.) Handbook of industrial, work & organizational psychology, Vol 1. Sage.

  • Latham, G. P., & Piccolo, R. F. (2012). The effect of context specific versus non-specific subconscious goals on employee performance. Human Resource Management, 51, 535–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P. & Seijts, G. H. (2016). Similarities and differences among performance, behavioral, and learning goals. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 1–9.

  • Latham, G. P., Stajkovic, A. D., & Locke, E. A. (2010). The relevance and viability of subconscious goals in the workplace. Journal of Management, 36, 234–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., Brcic, J., & Steinhauer, A. (2017). Toward an integration of goal setting theory and the automaticity model. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 66, 25–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A. (2015). Theory building, replication, and behavioral priming: Where do we need to go from here? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 408–414.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2004). What should we do about motivation theory? Six recommendations for the twenty-first century. Academy of Management Review, 29, 388–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (Eds.). (2013). New developments in goal setting and task performance. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maltarich, M.A. (2009). A first examination of the effects of conscious and subconscious goal commitment and conscious and subconscious achievement goals on task performance, Dissertation. The University of Wisconsin - Madison.

  • Mohrman, S. A., & Lawler, E. E. III. (2011). Useful research: Advancing theory and practice. San Francisco: Berett-Koehler Publishers, Inc.

  • Molden, D. C. (2014). Understanding priming effects in social psychology: An overview and integration. Social Cognition, 32, 243–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K. (2016). Health goal priming as a situated intervention tool: How to benefit from nonconscious motivational routes to health behavior. Health Psychology Review, 10, 408–424.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., Potjes, I., Keesman, M., Schwinghammer, S., & van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2014). Using health primes to reduce unhealthy snack purchases among overweight consumers in a grocery store. International Journal of Obesity, 38, 597–602.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Boyd, R. L., Jordan, K., & Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. Austin: University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfeffer, J., & Sutton, R. I. (2006). Hard facts, dangerous half-truths, and total nonsense: Profiting from evidence-based management. Harvard Business Press.

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, R. D., Young, B. L., Koenig, N., Schmerling, D., & Dixon, N. W. (2013). Long-term effects of goal setting on performance with the productivity measurement and enhancement system. In E. A. Locke, & G. P. Latham (Eds). New developments in goal setting and task performance (pp. 233–245). Routledge.

  • Reilly, B. M., Evans, T., Schaider, J. J., & Wang, Y. (2002). Triage of patients with chestpain in the emergency department: A comparative study of physicians’ decisions. American Journal of Medicine, 112, 95–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2015). Do subtle reminders of money change people’s political views? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, e73–e85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (2006). Matched sampling for causal effects. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). When opportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goals by means and its effects on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1109–1122.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shantz, A., & Latham, G. P. (2009). An exploratory field experiment of the effect of subconscious and conscious goals on employee performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 9–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shantz, A., & Latham, G. P. (2011). The effect of primed goals on employee performance: Implications for human resource management. Human Resource Management, 50, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., Verschuere, B., McCarthy, R. J., & O’Donnell, MM. (2018). New results from three multi-lab registered replication reports on priming effects. Presented at Association for Psychological Science Annual Convention, San Francisco, CA.

  • Sitzmann, T., & Bell, B. S. (2017). The dynamic effects of subconscious goal pursuit on resource allocation, task performance, and goal abandonment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 138, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stajkovic, A. D., & Locke, E. A. (2004). Goal priming: A new approach to goal research? Society for industrial and organizational psychology. Chicago, IL.

  • Stajkovic, A. D., Locke, E. A., & Blair, E. S. (2006). A first examination of the relationships between primed subconscious goals, assigned conscious goals, and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1172–1180.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tate, C. U. (2015). On the overuse and misuse of mediation analysis: It may be a matter of timing. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 37, 235–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59, 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vadillo, M. A., Hardwicke, T. E., & Shanks, D. R. (2016). Selection bias, vote counting, and money-priming effects: A comment on Rohrer, Pashler, and Harris (2015) and Vohs (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(5), 655–663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Yi, J., Hepler, J., & Albarracin, D. (2016). From primed effects to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 472–497.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, D., & Ordóñez, L. D. (2014). Conscience without cognition: The effects of subconscious priming on ethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 723–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winters, D., & Latham, G. P. (1996). The effect of learning versus outcome goals on a simple versus a complex task. Group & Organization Management, 21, 236–250.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexander D. Stajkovic.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 5 Sentence unscrambling task to prime achievement goal for experiment 2
Table 6 Funneled debriefing questions to probe for awareness in experiment 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stajkovic, A.D., Latham, G.P., Sergent, K. et al. Prime and Performance: Can a CEO Motivate Employees Without Their Awareness?. J Bus Psychol 34, 791–802 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-018-9598-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-018-9598-x

Keywords

Navigation