Introduction

Although empirical evidence does not consistently support the claim that teams enhance performance, there is a ‘romance of teams’ (Allen and Hecht 2004) involving a strong faith in the effectiveness of team-based work which has caused teams to become a prominent feature of the organizational landscape. Instead of relying on functional structures, many organizations are adopting team-based structures in which teams are responsible for key organizational outputs (Beyerlein et al. 1995; McDermott 1999). In some cases, these are whole products or services, whereas in other cases, they are sub-products or elements of the organization’s “value-chain” (Katzenbach and Smith 1993). Teams are supposed to stimulate the stronger need for flexibility, quality consciousness, and innovation (Anderson and Michael 1996; Lambert and Peppard 1993), because they give space for the creativity and problem solving competences of team members.

One of the expected yields of teamwork is the stimulation of team learning (Critchley and Casey 1989; Katzenbach and Smith 1993; Kofman and Senge 1993; Senge 1990; Tjosvold and Yu 2004). Teams bring together complementary skills and experience that exceed those of individual members in the team (Katzenbach and Smith 1993). Senge (1990) even claims that teams and not individuals are the fundamental learning unit in modern organizations: “unless the team can learn, the organization cannot learn” (p. 40). Whereas some authors define organizational learning or team learning as an outcome (Levitt and March 1988), others define learning as a process. Following Argyris and Schön (1996) and Edmondson (1999), we adhere to the latter approach and focus on the learning behaviors through which outcomes such as a better team performance can be achieved. More specifically, we focus on knowledge sharing behavior, as this is both a basic element of the team learning process (Wilson et al. 2007) and a condition for organizational learning (Hoopes and Postrel 1999; Nonaka and Takeuchi 1995). With an effective knowledge sharing process, organizations can develop their knowledge base and competitiveness (Andrews and Delahaye 2000; McEvily and Chakravarthy 2002).

Knowledge sharing within teams may occur via the advice-seeking behavior of team members (Podolny and Baron 1997). When team members ask for and give each other advice, they are likely to become more competent in task execution. In this study, we focus on the tendency of team members to ask for and give each other advice as an indicator for knowledge sharing within teams. Furthermore, we consider the openness of team members to share ideas and suggestions (Chow et al. 2000; Kasl et al. 1997) to be another important aspect of knowledge sharing.

Many gurus on organizational learning stress the importance of the role that disagreement can play in knowledge sharing or organizational learning (Argyris and Schön 1996; Senge 1990; Swieringa and Wierdsma 1990). In line with this, some studies confirm a positive effect from conflict on team learning (Ellis et al. 2003; Kasl et al. 1997; Van Offenbeek 2001). On the other hand, a meta-analysis shows negative relationships among conflict, team performance, and team member satisfaction (De Dreu and Weingart 2003). Moreover, disagreement within teams is likely to destroy the mutual trust and cooperation that is needed for effective knowledge sharing (Rastogi 2000). Therefore, in this study, we expect that, when team members disagree about ideas on what is important for their team, they are less likely to give and ask each other for advice and to listen to each others’ suggestions (Moye and Langfred 2004). As sharing knowledge with other team members is a voluntary and conscious act conducted by an individual, we expect that the cohesiveness in a team also plays a role in knowledge sharing. Cohesiveness within a team refers to the affinity between group members and their identification as a group (Organ et al. 2006). Team members are more willing to show cooperative behavior to each other and tend to be more willing to aid and assist team members if the ties that bind them are stronger (Cartwright 1968; Davis 1969; Mullen and Copper 1994; Schachter et al. 1951).

In this article, we begin with an examination of the relationship between disagreement and cohesiveness. We then explore the relationship between knowledge sharing behavior within teams and individual performance of team members. The conceptual model upon which this study is based is depicted in Fig. 1. Data for the study were gathered by means of a questionnaire, which was administered to 1,354 employees working in 126 teams within 17 Dutch organizations.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model

Knowledge Sharing Within Teams

In team contexts, people can learn, not only from their own direct experiences, but also from the experiences of other team members (Ickes and Gonzalez 1994; Jarvis 1995). Because team members can interact with one another, knowledge gathered by one team member can be transferred to his/her teammates through feedback, explanation, help, or advice (Ellis et al. 2003; Tjepkema 2003). Exchange of knowledge between team members brings knowledge sources together and manipulates it into new knowledge structures or routines (Clark et al. 2002). Van Offenbeek (2001) speaks in this context about the information distribution process, whereby team members distribute information from different sources to the other members of the team. Kasl et al. (1997) speak of individuals sharing information and perspectives in the interest of team efficiency and effectiveness. According to Dillen and Romme (1997) and Senge (1990), dialogue stimulates team members to think together, so that individual opinions play a role but are not decisive. Edmondson et al. (2001) found that cardiac surgery teams whose members felt comfortable making suggestions to each other were more successful in learning new procedures. When people felt uneasy acting this way, the learning process was stifled.

As learning from secondary experience seems to be the key element in team learning, this study focuses on team members’ ability to collectively share that experience with their team mates (Dillen and Romme 1997; Ellis et al. 2003) by asking and giving advice to each other and by being open to hearing ideas, opinions, or suggestions from each other (Kasl et al. 1997).

The Relationship Between Disagreement and Knowledge Sharing Within Teams

Despite the voluminous work on conflict, a clear generally accepted definition and typology of the construct is still lacking (Barki and Hartwick 2004; Wall and Callister 1995). Conflict can be seen as one of the challenges of working effectively in teams (De Dreu and Weingart 2003). Three properties can be identified as underlying many descriptions of what conflict is: disagreement, negative emotion, and interference (Barki and Hartwick 2004). More recently, researchers have identified task conflict, or cognitive conflict (Amason 1996), as one type of conflict. Although definitions of this construct have essentially focused on perceived disagreements, others include negative emotions (Jehn and Mannix 2001). For reasons of conceptual clearness, this study focuses on disagreement about ideas and goals.

The literature on the effects of disagreement or task conflict on team processes is inconclusive. On the one hand, some studies show that facile and uncritical agreement within the team can have a negative impact on problem solving (Aldag and Fuller 1993; Janis 1985), whereas conflict forces individuals to think more deeply and more creatively about the problem they have to solve. Van Offenbeek (2001) finds that the more divergent the ideas team members have about the task initially are, the more team members will experience having learned something at the end. Sauquet (2002) concludes that the inability to face conflict in a more global way makes creation of collective knowledge impossible. This is in line with De Dreu (1997), Turner and Pratkanis (1997), and Janis (1972), who argue that conflict suppression reduces individual creativity and decision-making quality in teams.

On the other hand, Stock (2004) concludes from a literature review based on 72 empirical studies investigating the antecedents of team performance that both positive and negative effects have been found for task-related conflicts. De Dreu and Weingart (2003) found a strong negative relationship between task conflict and team performance in their meta-analysis of 30 studies.

Although there are no studies available on the relation between disagreement and knowledge sharing activities, such as advice seeking behavior and openness for sharing opinions and suggestions, it is more likely that when team members disagree with each other they will be less inclined to ask and give advice to each other and to be open to hearing the ideas, opinions, and suggestions of team members. Therefore, it is hypothesized that disagreement is negatively related to asking and giving advice to each other (Hypothesis 1a) and that disagreement is negatively related to openness for sharing opinions and suggestions (Hypothesis 1b).

The Relationship Between Cohesiveness and Knowledge Sharing Within Teams

When team members undertake activities together, such as having lunch together, visiting each other at home, or having a drink after work, they get to know each other better and relations become stronger, making the team more cohesive (Sanders and Van Emmerik 2004). Cohesiveness increases the energy team members can devote to task-related activities because team maintenance needs are reduced. Highly cohesive teams experience less inter-member friction, higher member trust, and greater interpersonal coordination (Dobbins and Zaccaro 1986). Furthermore, research shows positive relationships between cohesiveness and employee satisfaction (Dobbins and Zaccaro 1986), as well as cohesiveness and cooperative behavior (Kidwell et al. 1997; Sanders and Van Emmerik 2004), and also shows negative relationships between cohesiveness and (short-term) absenteeism (Sanders 2004; Sanders and Hoekstra 1998). Shaw (1981) argues that members of highly cohesive teams are likely to be more motivated to achieve established team goals. Based on a survey of managers, Berman et al. (2002) show that workplace friendship increases openness, communication, and task accomplishment.

Given the above-mentioned research, it is likely that team cohesiveness plays a role in knowledge sharing within teams, as well. Research from workplace-learning behavior shows that individuals tend to attribute more of their learning to informal support of co-workers than to formal training provided by the organization (Maurer et al. 2003; Tannenbaum 1997). In this way, knowledge sharing within a team can be seen as cooperative behavior of team members, which has shown to be affected by the cohesiveness of a team. As sharing knowledge with other team members is a voluntary and conscious act on the part of an individual (Dixon 2002; Nonaka 1994), involving commitment from both transmitter and receiver (Michailova and Hutchings 2006), we hypothesize that cohesiveness in a team is positively related to asking and giving advice (Hypothesis 2a) and that cohesiveness in a team is positively related to openness for sharing opinions and suggestions (Hypothesis 2b).

Knowledge Sharing and Individual Performance

For the past decade, practitioners have pronounced the importance of knowledge sharing for organizational effectiveness. Yet, research has only begun to examine the empirical relationship between knowledge sharing and both team and individual performance (Druskat and Kayes 2000). Although empirical evidence does not consistently support the claim that teams enhance performance (Allen and Hecht 2004), some authors have found positive relationships. Bunderson and Sutcliffe’s (2003) study of management teams showed that an appropriate emphasis on learning has positive consequences for team effectiveness. Van Offenbeek (2001) found that the activities ‘information storage and retrieval’ and ‘acquiring information’ influenced the performance of student teams positively. Edmondson et al. (2001) also found evidence for a positive relationship between a team’s learning focus and observer ratings of overall team effectiveness in a sample of manufacturing teams. In this study, we will focus on individual performance instead of team performance. As it is likely that the performance of individual team members will also benefit from the advice and feedback from their team members (Kluger and DeNisi 1996; Moye and Langfred 2004; Pearsall and Ellis 2006; Tindale et al. 1991), we hypothesize that asking and giving advice is positively related to individual performance (Hypothesis 3a) and that openness for sharing opinions and suggestions is positively related to individual performance (Hypothesis 3b).

Summarizing the above hypotheses, we expect that knowledge sharing is an intermediate variable in the relationship between cohesiveness and disagreement, on the one hand, and individual performance, on the other hand. Consequently, we hypothesize that asking and giving advice mediates the relationship between cohesiveness (Hypothesis 4a) and disagreement (Hypothesis 4b), on the one hand, and individual performance, on the other hand. Furthermore, we hypothesize that openness for sharing opinions and suggestions mediates the relationship between cohesiveness (Hypothesis 4c) and disagreement (Hypothesis 4d), on the one hand, and individual performance, on the other hand.

Method

Respondents

We employed a cross-sectional design in which we sampled teams from a wide range of organizations both in the public sector (three faculties of a university, a ministry, departments of the royal air force, a nursing home, a swimming pool) and in the private sector (a consultancy firm, some small manufacturing organizations). Although our sample consists of a diversity of teams, all respondents participated in ongoing teams, with long task duration, and involving between 5 and 15 team members. Although the task performed by the individual teams differed, in all cases, the team members needed each other to complete the team’s product or service.

Questionnaires were distributed to a total of 3,312 respondents. The overall response rate was 45%. The response rate differed between organizations (between 21 and 90%) and between teams (between 5 and 100%). Due to the fact that some measures were based on team measures, only data from employees for which there were at least five respondents from their team were retained, as past research has indicated that biases in using aggregate scores begin to diminish with groups of five or more employees (Bliese 2000). As teams with less than five respondents were excluded from our sample, our final sample consisted of 1,354 respondents from 126 teams in 17 Dutch organizations, with an average group size of 10.7 responding workers per team.

60% of the respondents were male; the mean age was 40.8 years (SD = 11.76). 77% of the respondents had a higher education background. Most employees (76%) had a permanent contract, 8% had a temporary job with the perspective of a permanent job in the near future, and 8% had a temporary job. The respondents had, on average, 12.2 years work experience within the current organization (SD = 9.7) and 5.5 years work experience in the current position (SD = 5.12). The mean total work experience was 16.9 years (SD = 10.9). The respondents worked, on average, 27.9 h a week (SD = 14.4).

Procedure

Before the questionnaires were distributed, stage meetings were held with the board of directors and the managers of all the organizations (see Lambooij et al. 2002). The relevant unions and works councils were also informed of the goals, design, and possible consequences of the research. Thereafter, all employees were informed about the research and the way the data would be collected. In all of the different organizations, the data were collected by master students who used the data from the specific organization for writing a master thesis. Within all organizations, these students personally handed over the questionnaires and reply envelopes to the employees. The respondents were asked to send the completed questionnaire by means of the reply envelope to the university. Employees were told that data would be collected about their experiences working within a team. Employees answered the questionnaire voluntarily; there were no consequences for answering the questionnaire, neither positive nor negative. These activities contributed to the relatively high survey response rate of 45%, with no discernible difference between respondents and non-respondents (Buffinga 2004; Jonker 2003). The sample’s demographic (gender, age, education, type of labor contract, and number of working hours) show similarities with the Dutch working population (CBS 2004, 2005), although not in all respects.Footnote 1 The dataset over-represents employees with a higher vocational education background working in public sector organizations.Footnote 2

Measures

All items of the scales that were used in this study were measured with a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). An exception to this was the cohesiveness scale that consists of seven points with different anchors (1 = nobody, 7 = everyone). The items that comprise the different scales are included in Appendix.

Disagreement was measured with a 3-item scale that is part of the outcome interdependence scale of Van der Vegt et al. (1998). An example is “In my team we often have disagreements concerning ideas.” The reliability of this 3-item scale was good (α = .91).

Cohesiveness within a team was measured with a scale adapted from Dobbins and Zaccaro (1986), which consists of eight items concerning team members’ willingness to share activities and to have personal talks. Examples of the items were “With how many people in your team do you occasionally talk about personal things?” and “With how many people in your team did you engage in one of the following activities: went for dinner, went to the movies, went visiting?” Answers ranged from 1 = nobody, to 7 = everybody. The scale was reliable (α = .90).

For knowledge sharing, two measures were used. First, we measured the extent to which team members ask and give advice to their team members with a self-developed 5-item scale. Examples of these questions are “Colleagues ask me for my advice regularly,” and “I regularly ask my supervisor for advice.” Although the reliability of this scale was sufficient but not high (α = .71), based on theoretical grounds, we decided to keep all five items within in the scale.

Secondly, to measure the openness for sharing opinions and suggestions, three items of the scale developed by Costa (2000) were used. Examples of items are “In team meetings, people are reluctant to give their opinion” and “Most people in my team are not interested in ideas or suggestions made by others.” The reliability of the scale was good (α = .84). For this study, items were recoded so that low scores indicate little openness and high scores indicate high openness.

To measure individual performance, we used four items from the subjective performance scale part of organizational citizenship behavior (MacKenzie et al. 1991). Examples of items are “I consider myself one of the best employees of this organization,” and “If I compare myself to my near colleagues, I am better in my job” (α = .77).

Control Variables

As various studies have indicated that the positive effects of teams depend on work, worker, and team characteristics (Barrick et al. 1998; Campion et al. 1996; Stewart and Barrick 2000), we included job, employee, and team characteristics as control variables.

Job Characteristics

Respondents were asked about their labor contract (0 = permanent labor contract, 1 = temporary labor contract) and number of working hours a week. The autonomy and responsibility of the job were asked with a single item “How much autonomy/responsibility do you have in your job.” Answers could be given on a 7-point scale (1 = low, 7 = high).

Employee Characteristics

Respondents were asked to answer questions concerning gender (0 = male, 1 = female), age (“year of birth,” which was recoded), level of education on a 7-point scale of educational qualifications (1 = secondary school, 7 = post graduate university level), and number of years within organization.

Team Characteristics

We have added team size to our analyses, because previous studies (Mohammed and Angell 2004; Steiner 1972; Wilke and Meertens 1994) have shown this variable to be relevant to team effectiveness. For this control variable, we used the number of team members and not the number of respondents from the different teams.

Analysis

The measures of disagreement and cohesiveness that are used in this study refer to characteristics of teams instead of characteristics of individual employees. For instance, a team is more or less cohesive, not an employee. Asking and giving advice and openness, on the other hand, are interpreted in this study as perceptions of the employees and taken into account on the individual level. Because of this, aggregation characteristics (values of ICC1 and ICC2; Bliese 2000) were calculated for disagreement and cohesiveness. The values of ICC1 are similar to what is found in the research literature (e.g., values in the range of .05 to .15; disagreement: .15; cohesiveness: .12). Given the number of groups in the study, we can assume there is enough agreement within groups to make our study feasible. Values of ICC2 above .50 are acceptable (Klein and Kozlowski 2000), whereas values above .70 are considered good. The ICC2 for disagreement is .58, and for cohesiveness, .64. This means that the different scales intended for this study are acceptable on a team level.

The dataset consisted of employees nested in teams, which were in turn nested in organizations. Because the variance of the different dependent variables (asking and giving advice, openness, and individual performance) is hardly related to the organization level (ranging from .01 to .19%), this level is not taken into account. This means that the data can be conceptualized at two levels (employee and teams). Level 1 captures the information of the employees in each team (the two aspects of knowledge sharing for the first analyses and the perceived individual performance for the second analyses), and level 2 captures the variability between teams (disagreement and cohesiveness). In such situations, it is appropriate to use a hierarchical 2-level modeling approach that simultaneously models effects at the within- and between subunit-level (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). We used the statistical package MLWIN V1.02 to analyze our two-level data.

The data were analyzed using hierarchical regression analyses. First, the mediating variables (asking and giving advice and openness) were regressed on the independent variables (disagreement and cohesiveness). The results of these analyses are presented in Table 2. Second, the dependent variable individual performance was regressed on the independent and mediating variables (Table 3).

Results

Table 1 provides the means, standard deviations, and correlations for all variables included in this study. As we can see, there was a significant positive relationship between cohesiveness and openness (r = .32, p < .01). There were strong significant negative relationships between disagreement and asking and giving advice (r = −.45, p < .01) and between disagreement and openness (r = −.37, p < .01). Further, asking and giving advice had a positive relationship with perceived individual performance (r = .11, p < .05).

Table 1 Descriptives and correlations between variables (n = 1,354)

From the correlations with the control variables, we can see that a significant positive correlation exists for both job autonomy and job responsibility with respect to individual performance, asking and giving advice, and openness. The number of years in the organization relates significantly positive with openness, and significantly negative with disagreement. Females have significantly higher values for individual performance and for openness. Age relates significantly positive with individual performance and disagreement. Educational level relates significantly negative with individual performance and also significantly negative with disagreement. Team size relates significantly positive with perceived individual performance, but significantly negative with asking and giving advice, openness, cohesiveness, and disagreement.

To test the first and second hypotheses, we turn to the results of the hierarchical regression analyses with the individual-level-dependent variables ‘asking and giving advice’ and ‘openness’ reported in Table 2. As we can see in Table 2, Hypothesis 1a, which predicts a negative relationship between disagreement and asking and giving advice, could not be supported (B = −.17, n.s.). However, a negative relationship was indeed found between disagreement and openness (B = −.28, p < .01), so that Hypothesis 1b could be supported. Furthermore, as we can see in Table 2, Hypotheses 2a and 2b, which predict a positive relationship between cohesiveness, on the one hand, and asking and giving advice and openness, on the other hand, could both be supported (B = .09, p < .05; and B = .48; p < .01).

Table 2 Results of regression analyses with asking and giving advice and openness as dependent variables (multi level analyses; n = 1,354)

The results of the regression analyses with individual performance as dependent variable are reported in Table 3. As we can see in Table 3 (model 1), Hypothesis 3a concerning the relationship between asking and giving advice and individual performance is supported (B = .42, p < .01). There is, however, no support for Hypothesis 3b concerning the relationship between openness and individual performance (B = .11, n.s.).

Table 3 Results of regression analyses with individual performance as dependent variable (n = 1,354)

Hypothesis 4a concerned the mediating role of asking and giving advice in the relationship between cohesiveness and perceived individual performance. This hypothesis can be confirmed as cohesiveness showed to have a significant effect on asking and giving advice (B = .09, p < .05), asking and giving advice has a significant effect on individual performance (B = .39, p < .01), and cohesiveness has a significant relationship with individual performance that disappears when asking and giving advice is added to the model (B = .05, n.s.). As there is no significant effect from disagreement on asking and giving advice (B = −.17, n.s.), this latter variable does not have a mediating effect in the relationship between disagreement and individual performance, so that Hypothesis 4b cannot be confirmed. Furthermore, Hypothesis 4c concerning the mediating role of openness in the relationship between cohesiveness and perceived individual performance and Hypothesis 4d concerning the mediating role of openness in the relationship between disagreement and individual performance cannot be confirmed as there is no effect from openness on individual performance. Figures 2 and 3 show our empirical findings regarding the conceptual model.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Empirical results for asking and giving advice

Fig. 3
figure 3

Empirical results for openness for sharing opinions and suggestions

Conclusions

In this study, we investigated the relationships between disagreement and cohesiveness in teams, on the one hand, and knowledge sharing in teams and individual performance, on the other. As teams have become a prominent feature of the organizational landscape, and as organizations have to ‘learn’ in order to successfully compete, it is important to know what conditions stimulate knowledge-sharing behavior in teams.

Although many authors report a positive effect from disagreement on group processes such as knowledge sharing and team learning (Ellis et al. 2003; Kasl et al. 1997; Van Offenbeek 2001), this study does not confirm these results. The results show that there is a significant negative main effect from disagreement on openness for sharing ideas and suggestions. Perhaps the expected positive effect of disagreement on knowledge sharing is part of the ‘romance of teams’ (Allen and Hecht 2004).

Our results are, however, in line with a meta-analysis conducted by De Dreu and Weingart (2003), in which strong and negative (instead of the predicted positive) correlations between task conflict and team performance and a less negative effect of task conflict on team performance when task conflict and relation conflict were weakly correlated were found. De Dreu and Weingart (2003) suggest that the fact that results showed no differential relationship between type of conflict and team performance might be caused by a measurement problem, since most research included in their analysis relied on the scale developed by Jehn (1994, 1995). However, as we used a self-developed scale for disagreement, and, as we found negative effects from this variable on knowledge sharing within teams, this explanation seems less plausible. More plausible is that high levels of disagreement can stifle knowledge sharing by creating barriers in communication (Tjepkema 2003), harming interpersonal understanding (Druskat and Kayes 2000), or producing affective conflicts (Devine 1999). Another explanation might be that the relation between disagreement and knowledge sharing is an inverted U-shaped relationship (Stock 2004). Studies (Jehn 1995; Stock 2004) based on nonlinear models have suggested that both too few and too intensive task-related conflicts reduce team performance. The same may apply to knowledge sharing: only when there are neither too many nor too few diverging views in a team will each team member be open to new ideas and enter into a cognitive mode that allows for the questioning of assumptions and the generation of new insights (Driver 2003).

Our study did confirm the importance of cohesiveness for knowledge sharing and the importance of knowledge sharing as an intermediate variable between cohesiveness, on the one hand, and team performance, on the other hand. Our study shows that asking and giving advice within teams can be seen as a pure mediating variable in the relationship between cohesiveness and the individual performance of team members. Team cohesiveness in and of itself is not enough to enhance individual performance.

The results of this study are in line with the results reported by Edmondson (1999), who claims a positive effect from team psychological safety on team-learning behavior. However, Edmondson warns that team psychological safety is not the same as group cohesiveness, as earlier research (Janis 1982) has shown that cohesiveness can reduce willingness to disagree and challenge others’ views, as in the phenomenon of groupthink, which implies a lack of interpersonal risk-taking. Our study shows nevertheless that the positive sides of team cohesiveness are probably stronger than its negative sides, as we have found no significant relationship between cohesiveness and disagreement and as we have found a positive effect from cohesiveness on knowledge sharing within teams. This means that team members in cohesive teams disagree just as much with each other as team members in non-cohesive teams, but that they do share more knowledge with each other, probably because they feel more motivated to achieve established team goals.

This study has limitations and strengths. The first limitation is that we used a cross-sectional design, which makes it impossible to draw conclusions about the causality of relationships. Future research into knowledge sharing in teams using a longitudinal approach should shed more light on this. A second limitation is that we measured the perception of individual performance instead of some objective measure for team performance. In a following study, more objective measures of both individual and team performance should be included. Another possible avenue for future research is to not only look for existence of disagreement, but also for the ability of teams to manage conflicts (De Reuver 2006; Shaw and Barrett-Power 1998). Strength of this study is that it focuses on knowledge-sharing behavior as a mediating variable between cohesiveness and disagreement, on the one hand, and individual performance of team members, on the other hand. While the effects of conflict on group performance have been studied and discussed extensively (De Dreu and Weingart 2003), the relationship between conflict and knowledge sharing has not been explored fully (Moye and Langfred 2004). This study is certainly not the final word on the relationship between disagreement and knowledge sharing. More studies in the proposed directions are needed in order to refine and possibly replicate our findings. Our study does indicate, however, that the relationship between disagreement and collective learning that is stressed by some gurus in organizational learning is a complex relationship. Sorting out how relationships between team members influence the learning behavior and the performance of individuals and teams may be a key element for understanding collective learning processes in organizations.