Skip to main content
Log in

Spontaneous Response Generalization During Whole Word Instruction: Reading to Spell and Spelling to Read

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The bulk of the existing literature emphasizes the use of phonemic/phonetic based instruction to enhance generalization between reading and written language. However, phonetically irregular words are common in English and may require the use of whole word approaches. This study examined generalization between from reading to spelling and from spelling to reading following whole word based instruction using a delayed prompt procedure. The total instructional time was equated across conditions. Students exhibited substantial but incomplete generalization with the degree of generalization varying somewhat across responses and students. The most striking finding was that oral spelling instruction in which the printed word was neither presented nor produced resulted in substantial generalization to reading. These data illustrate a model for examining response generalization in instructional contexts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alig-Cybriwsky, A., Wolery, M., & Gast, D. L. (1990). Use of a constant time delay procedure in teaching preschoolers in a group format. Journal of Early Intervention, 14, 99–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (1987). Applied Behavior Analysis. Upper saddle River: Prentice Hall.

  • de Rose, J. C., de Souza, D. G., & Hanna, E. S. (1996). Teaching reading and spelling: Exclusion and stimulus equivalence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 451–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. (1993). Links among segmenting, spelling, and reading words in first and second grades. Reading & Writing, 5, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harper, G. F., Mallette, B., Maheady, L., Parkes, V. & Moore, J. (1993). Retention and generalization of spelling words acquired using a peer-mediated procedure by children with mild handicapping conditions. Journal of Behavioral Education, 3, 25–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, L. M. (2000). Developing first graders’ phonemic awareness, word identification and spelling: A comparison of two contemporary phonics instructional approaches. Reading Research & Instruction, 39, 160–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, B. P. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

  • O’Connor, R. E., & Jenkins, J. R. (1995). Improving the generalization of sound/symbol knowledge: Teaching spelling to kindergarten children with disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 29, 255–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, R. E., & Padeliadu, S. (2000). Blending versus whole word approaches in first grade remedial reading: Short-term and delayed effects on reading and spelling words. Reading & Writing, 13, 159–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okyere, B. A., Heron, T. E., & Goddard, Y. (1997). Effects of self-correction on the acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of the written spelling of elementary school children. Journal of Behavioral Education, 7, 51–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereira, J. A., & Winton, A. S. W. (1991). Teaching and remediation of mathematics: A review of behavioral research. Journal of Behavioral Education, 1, 5–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., Johnston, M. D., & Brady, N. C. (2000). Identity matching of consonant-vowel-consonant words by prereaders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 309–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Willson-Morris, M., & Kirk, B. (1986). Match-to-sample procedures and the development of equivalence relations: The role of naming. Analysis & Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

  • Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. B. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to George H. Noell.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Noell, G.H., Connell, J.E. & Duhon, G.J. Spontaneous Response Generalization During Whole Word Instruction: Reading to Spell and Spelling to Read. J Behav Educ 15, 121–130 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-006-9016-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-006-9016-8

Keywords

Navigation