Skip to main content
Log in

Stimulated Variation and Cascades: Two Processes in the Evolution of Complex Technological Systems

  • Published:
Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Michael Schiffer’s theoretical and methodological contributions to archaeology are substantial. For the last two decades, Schiffer has become increasingly interested in the history of electrical technology, including portable radios, electric automobiles, eighteenth-century electrostatic technology, and, most recently, nineteenth-century electric light and power systems. Schiffer has long held a behavioral view, which focuses analytical attention on interactions between humans and material things, including complex technological systems (CTSs). For Schiffer, two key aspects of the evolution of CTSs are stimulated variation, defined as an increase in invention resulting from changing selective conditions, and cascading, defined as sequential spurts of invention that occur through the recognition of emergent performance problems in a CTS. To attain maximum usefulness, these concepts should be placed in a modern evolutionary framework that correctly identifies, and does not oversell, the role played by cultural selection. Research on individual and social learning provides the critical link between Schiffer’s stimulated variation and cascade models and the diffusion of CTSs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. As Wimsatt (1999, p. 283) noted, the seeming arbitrariness of cultural traits as cultural fragments provides “our ability to re-package and re-articulate cultural products into seemingly arbitrary larger or smaller constructions to be replicated and transmitted as units.” In other words, “most cultural products are also compound products” (Wimsatt 1999, p. 285)—a characteristic not lost on early ethnologists. Driver and Kroeber (1932, p. 213), for example, had this to say: “Are our elements or factors, the culture traits, independent of one another? While we are not prepared to answer this question categorically, we believe that culture traits are in the main if not in absolutely all cases independent…. Essential parts of a trait cannot of course be counted as separate traits: the stern of a canoe, the string of a bow, etc. Even the bow and arrow is a single trait until there is question of an arrow-less bow. Then, we have two traits, the pellet bow and arrow bow.” Similarly, Barnett (1953, p. 356) remarked, there are “persistent linkages between idea-sets as they diffuse across ethnic boundaries. Artifacts of this sort are called complexes because the analyst finds them to be made up of more than one component.”

  2. Several archaeological studies have drawn inspiration in part from Schiffer’s treatment of stimulated variation and the cascade model. For example, Lyman and O’Brien (2000) used a variety of data sets, including Schiffer’s (1996) radio data, to show the usefulness of clade-diversity diagrams for exploring the origination of novel variants and explaining the history of artifact lineages. Similarly, Lyman et al. (2008, 2009) and VanPool et al. (n.d.) used clade-diversity diagrams to examine the evolution of prehistoric weapon-delivery systems in western North America, beginning with the atlatl and dart and ending with the bow and arrow.

  3. There is some evidence that S-shaped curves can arise through a number of plausible asocial processes as well, even if the assumption of a well-mixed population with no spatial heterogeneity in resources is accepted (Hoppitt et al. 2010). This possibility does not affect discussion here.

  4. If indeed there is one. The challenge of this approach is in defining the full landscape of possible inventions. Ancient Polynesians built ocean-going canoes, but they could not have invented jet skis. We might wonder, though, what else was on the Polynesian design landscape that they might have ventured upon?

  5. There is an extensive literature in the social sciences on computing the costs of learning. For an early example, using utility curves, see Friedman and Savage (1948). For a useful summary from the perspective of human behavioral ecology, see Fitzhugh (2001).

References

  • Arthur, W. B. (2009). The nature of technology: What it is and how it evolves. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, H. G. (1953). Innovation: The basis of cultural change. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bass, F. M. (1969). A new product growth model for consumer durables. Management Science, 15, 215–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M., & Paciotti, B. M. (2004). Cultural evolution in laboratory microsocieties including traditions of rule giving and rule following. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 305–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beinhocker, E. (2006). The origin of wealth: Evolution, complexity, and the radical remaking of economics. Cambridge: Harvard Business School Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., Earls, M., & O’Brien, M. J. (2011). I’ll have what she’s having: Mapping social behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., & Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 1443–1450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2011). The selectivity of social learning and the tempo of cultural evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 9, (in press).

  • Bentley, R. A., & Ormerod, P. (2010). A rapid method for assessing social versus independent interest in health issues: A case study of ‘bird flu’ and ‘swine flu. Social Science & Medicine, 71, 482–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., & Shennan, S. J. (2003). Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity, 68, 459–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. (2010). Memetics does provide a useful way of understanding cultural evolution. In F. Ayala & R. Arp (Eds.), Contemporary debates in philosophy of biology (pp. 255–272). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). How microevolutionary processes give rise to history. In M. H. Nitecki & D. V. Nitecki (Eds.), History and evolution (pp. 179–209). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, B., & Hamilton, M. J. (2009). A formal test of the origin of variation in North American early Paleoindian projectile points. American Antiquity, 74, 279–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosh, A., Hughes, A., & Wood, E. (1996). Innovation in UK SMEs: Causes and the consequences for firm failure and acquisition. Cambridge: University of Cambridge, ESRC Centre for Business Research, Working Paper no. 48.

  • C., Darwin. (1868). The variation of animals and plants under domestication (vol. 2). London: Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, H. E., & Kroeber, A. L. (1932). Quantitative expression of cultural relationships. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology, 31, 211–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007a). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 649–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007b). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317, 1344–1347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W. (2000). Practice makes within 5% of perfect: The role of visual perception, motor skills, and human memory in artifact variation and standardization. Current Anthropology, 41, 663–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W., & Lipo, C. P. (2005). Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation of variation in material culture and the archaeological record. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 24, 316–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Critical social learning: A solution to Rogers’ paradox of non-adaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109, 727–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Strimling, P., Eriksson, K., Laland, K., & Sjostrand, J. (2010). One cultural parent makes no culture. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1353–1362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., Aoki, K., & Kumm, J. (1996). Individual versus social learning: Evolutionary analysis in a fluctuating environment. Anthropological Science, 104, 209–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (1996). Gene-culture co-evolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11, 453–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J., & Hinde, R. A. (1949). The opening of milk bottles by birds. British Birds, 42, 347–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzhugh, B. (2001). Risk and invention in human technological evolution. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 20, 125–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, M., & Nunn, C. L. (2009). Rapid evolution of social learning. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22, 1914–1922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, M., & Savage, L. J. (1948). The utility analysis of choices involving risk. Journal of Political Economy, 56, 279–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L. (2008). Mind. In R. A. Bentley, H. D. G. Maschner, & C. Chippendale (Eds.), Handbook of archaeology theories (pp. 283–296). London: AltaMira.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets, S. (1997). Evolution and speciation on holey adaptive landscapes. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 12, 307–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J., & Templeton, J. J. (2002). Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 357, 1559–1566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. (2000). The tipping point: how little things can make a big difference. New York: Little, Brown.

  • Gladwin, C., & Butler, J. (1984). Is gardening an adaptive strategy for Florida family farmers? Human Organization, 43, 208–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, M. J., & Buchanan, B. (2009). The accumulation of stochastic copying errors causes drift in culturally transmitted technologies: Quantifying Clovis evolutionary dynamics. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 28, 55–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist, 103, 992–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: Why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity, 69, 197–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2006). Understanding cultural evolutionary models: A reply to Read’s critique. American Antiquity, 71, 771–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2010). The evolution of innovation-enhancing institutions. In M. J. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 99–120). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 366, 1139–1148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiscox, G. (1900). Horseless vehicles, automobiles, motorcycles operated by steam, hydro-carbon, electric, and pneumatic motors. New York: Munn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt, W., Kandler, A., Kendal, J. R., & Laland, K. N. (2010). The effect of task structure on diffusion dynamics: Implications for diffusion curve and network-based analyses. Learning & Behavior, 38, 243–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2002). Cost-benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment: An evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 373–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2003). Does social/cultural learning increase human adaptability? Rogers’ question revisited. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 242–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandler, A., & Steele, J. (2010). Social learning, economic inequality, and innovation diffusion. In M. J. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 193–214). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, D. (1996). Local hillclimbing on an economic landscape. Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 96-08-065. Santa Fe, NM: Santa Fe Institute.

  • Kauffman, S. (1995). At home in the universe: The search for laws of self-organization and complexity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kauffman, S., Lobo, J., & Macready, W. (2000). Optimal search on a technology landscape. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 43, 141–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawai, M. (1965). Newly acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese macaques. Primates, 6, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, J., Giraldeau, L.-A., & Laland, K. N. (2009). The evolution of social learning rules: Payoff-based and frequency-dependent biased transmission. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 260, 210–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keynes, J. M. (1937). The general theory of employment. Quarterly Review of Economics, 51, 209–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M., & Boyd, R. (2010). Population size predicts tool complexity in Oceania. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 2559–2564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lake, M. W., & Venti, J. (2009). Quantitative analysis of macroevolutionary patterning in technological evolution: Bicycle design from 1800 to 2000. In S. J. Shennan (Ed.), Pattern and process in cultural evolution (pp. 147–162). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32, 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Atton, N., & Webster, M. M. (2011). From fish to fashion: Experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 958–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Galef, B. G. (Eds.). (2009). The question of animal culture. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Reader, S. M. (2010). Comparative perspectives on human innovation. In M. J. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 37–51). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1994). Cultural transmission in pigeons is affected by the number of tutors and bystanders present. Animal Behaviour, 47, 331–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, C. (1959). The science of ‘muddling through’. Public Administration Review, 19, 79–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C. P., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., & Shennan, S. J. (Eds.). (2006). Mapping our ancestors: Phylogenetic approaches in anthropology and prehistory. New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobo, J., & Macready, W. G. (1999). Landscapes: a natural extension of search theory. Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 99-05-037E. Santa Fe, NM: Santa Fe Institute.

  • Lowery, S. A., & DeFleur, M. L. (1995). Milestones in mass communication research. White Plains, NY: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., & O’Brien, M. J. (2000). Measuring and explaining change in artifact variation with clade-diversity diagrams. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 19, 39–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., & O’Brien, M. J. (2003). Cultural traits: Units of analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of Anthropological Research, 59, 225–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., VanPool, T. L., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008). Variation in North American dart points and arrow points when one or both are present. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 2805–2812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., VanPool, T. L., & O’Brien, M. J. (2009). The diversity of North American projectile-point types, before and after the bow and arrow. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 28, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., Holden, C., & Shennan, S. J. (Eds.). (2006). The evolution of cultural diversity: A phylogenetic approach. London: University College London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., & Jordan, F. M. (2011). Macro-evolutionary studies of cultural diversity: A review of empirical studies of cultural transmission and cultural adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 402–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., & Pagel, M. (1994). The comparative method in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 35, 549–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., Waring, T. M., Baum, W., Edsten, E., et al. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 483–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2007). Using the methods of social psychology to study cultural evolution. Journal of Social, Evolutionary and Cultural Psychology, 1, 35–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 350–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2010). The experimental study of cultural innovation. In M. J. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 175–191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011a). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian evolutionary theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011b). An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: Payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but under-used. Evolution and Human Behavior, in press.

  • Mesoudi, A., & Lycett, S. J. (2009). Random copying, frequency-dependent copying and culture change. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 41–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008a). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 73, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008b). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology II: An agent-based computer simulation. American Antiquity, 73, 627–724.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008c). The learning and transmission of hierarchical cultural recipes. Biological Theory, 3, 63–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple uses of cultural transmission experiments in understanding cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3489–3501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mom, G. (2004). The electric vehicle: Technology and expectations in the automobile age. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neiman, F. D. (1995). Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: Inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic assemblages. American Antiquity, 60, 7–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Holland, T. D. (1995). Behavioral archaeology and the extended phenotype. In J. M. Skibo, W. H. Walker, & A. E. Nielsen (Eds.), Expanding archaeology (pp. 143–161). Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2002). Evolutionary archaeology: Current status and future prospects. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 26–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., Mesoudi, A., & VanPool, T. L. (2010). Cultural traits as units of analysis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 3797–3806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Shennan, S. J. (Eds.). (2010). Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ormerod, P. (2006). Why most things fail: Evolution, extinction and economics. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennisi, E. (2010). Cultural evolution: Conquering by copying. Science, 328, 165–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petroski, H. (1992). The evolution of useful things: How everyday artifacts—from forks and pins to paper clips and zippers—came to be as they are. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 1298–1301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Biro, D. (2010). Experimental identification of social learning in wild animals. Learning & Behavior, 38, 265–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 4436–4441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Cownden, D., Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., Feldman, M. W., et al. (2010). Why copy others? Insights from the Social Learning Strategies Tournament. Science, 328, 208–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Enquist, M., Feldman, M. W., Fogarty, L., & Laland, K. N. (2011). How copying affects the amount, evenness and persistence of cultural knowledge: Insights from the Social Learning Strategies Tournament. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1118–1128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J. E., Morgan, T. J. H., Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renfrew, C., & Scarre, C. (Eds.). (1998). Cognition and material culture: The archaeology of symbolic storage. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2000). Climate, culture, and the evolution of cognition. In C. Heyes & L. Huber (Eds.), Evolution of congition (pp. 329–346). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 197, 8985–8992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieucau, G., & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2011). Exploring the costs and benefits of social information use: An appraisal of current experimental evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 949–957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of innovations (4th ed.). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, B., & Gross, N. C. (1943). The diffusion of hybrid seed corn in two Iowa communities. Rural Sociology, 8, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1990). The influence of surface treatment on heating effectiveness of ceramic vessels. Journal of Archaeological Science, 17, 373–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1991). The portable radio in American life. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1993). Cultural imperatives and product development: The case of the shirt-pocket radio. Technology and Culture, 34, 98–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1996). Some relationships between behavioral and evolutionary archaeologies. American Antiquity, 61, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2005a). The devil is in the details: The cascade model of invention processes. American Antiquity, 70, 485–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2005b). The electric lighthouse in the nineteenth century: Aid to navigation and political technology. Technology and Culture, 46, 275–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2008a). A cognitive analysis of component-stimulated invention: Electromagnet, telegraph, and the Capitol dome’s gas lighter. Technology and Culture, 49, 376–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2008b). Power struggles: Scientific authority and the creation of practical electricity before Edison. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2010). Can archaeologists study processes of invention? In M. J. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 235–249). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., Butts, T. C., & Grimm, K. K. (1994). Taking charge: The electric automobile in America. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., Hollenback, K. L., & Bell, C. L. (2003). Draw the lightning down: Benjamin Franklin and electrical technology in the Age of Enlightenment. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., & Skibo, J. M. (1987). Theory and experiment in the study of technological change. Current Anthropology, 28, 595–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., & Skibo, J. M. (1989). A provisional theory of ceramic abrasion. American Anthropologist, 91, 101–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., & Skibo, J. M. (1997). The explanation of artifact variability. American Antiquity, 62, 27–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schumpeter, J. A. (1942). Capitalism, socialism, and democracy. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shenkar, O. (2010). Copycats: How smart companies use imitation to gain a strategic edge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Business Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2002). Genes, memes and human history. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (Ed.). (2009). Pattern and process in cultural evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2011). Descent with modification and the archaeological record. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 36, 1070–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skibo, J. M., Schiffer, M. B., & Reid, K. C. (1989). Organic-tempered pottery: An experimental study. American Antiquity, 54, 122–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, T. E., & Podolny, J. M. (1996). Local search and the evolution of technological capabilities. Strategic Management Journal, 17, 21–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Surowiecki, J. (2004). The wisdom of crowds: Why the many are smarter than the few. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2405–2415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiselton-Dyer, W. T. (1895). Variation and specific stability. Nature, 51, 459–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Peer, Y., Maere, S., & Meyer, A. (2009). The evolutionary significance of ancient genome duplications. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, 725–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanPool, T. L., O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2011). Innovation and natural selection in Paleoindian projectile points from the American Southwest. In N. Goodale & W. Andrefsky Jr. (Eds.), Lithic technological systems: Stone, human behavior, evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. J. (2002). A simple model of global cascades on random networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 5766–5771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, J. H., Jr. (1997). American locomotives: An engineering history, 1830–1880 (rev. ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (2005). The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature, 437, 52–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Culture evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 938–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt, W. C. (1999). Genes, memes and cultural identity. Biology and Philosophy, 14, 279–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziman, J. (Ed.). (2000). Technological innovation as an evolutionary process. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Jim Skibo and Jeff Reid for their kind invitation to MJO to participate in the Society for American Archaeology symposium on Mike Schiffer’s work; the Leverhulme Trust (U.K.) for funding the “Tipping Points” program at Durham University; Melody Galen for creating the figures; and a reviewer whose thoughtful comments greatly improved the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. O’Brien.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

O’Brien, M.J., Bentley, R.A. Stimulated Variation and Cascades: Two Processes in the Evolution of Complex Technological Systems. J Archaeol Method Theory 18, 309–335 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-011-9110-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-011-9110-7

Keywords

Navigation