Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Evolutionary Foraging Models in Zooarchaeological Analysis: Recent Applications and Future Challenges

  • Original paper
  • Published:
Journal of Archaeological Research Aims and scope

Abstract

The last few decades have witnessed a rapid rise in the use of foraging models derived from behavioral ecology to explain and predict temporal and spatial differences in faunal assemblages. Although these models build on conventional ideas about utility firmly embedded in zooarchaeological analyses, when cast in an evolutionary framework these ideas produce some of the most sophisticated and elegant interpretations of archaeofaunas to date. In this article I review the methodological and practical strengths and weaknesses of current zooarchaeological applications of foraging models. Recent applications of foraging models to the zooarchaeological record reveal important variability in human-prey interactions across time and space. Case-specific applications generate theoretical and methodological advances that augment and are complementary to model building in allied fields. Recent applications also identify shortcomings in the underlying assumptions and rationale of some foraging models that mirror past and on-going discussions in anthropology and biology. I discuss how these shortcomings can fruitfully direct future applications and research in foraging economics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References cited

  • Alvard, M. S. (1993). Testing the ecologically noble savage hypothesis-interspecific prey choice by Piro hunters of Amazonian Peru. Human Ecology 21: 355–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M. S. (1994). Conservation by native peoples: Prey choice in a depleted habitat. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 5: 127–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M. S. (1995). Intraspecific prey choice by Amazonian hunters. Current Anthropology 36: 789–818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M. S., and Gillespie, A. (2004). Good Lamalera whale hunters accrue reproductive benefits. Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 225–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. J. (1981). A model of prehistoric collecting on the rocky shore. Journal Archaeological Science 8: 109–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. (2002). Faunal collapse, landscape change and settlement history in remote Oceania. World Archaeology 33: 375–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, K. G., Tracy, M. C., and de Queiroz, A. (2000). Is Bergmann’s rule valid for mammals? American Naturalist 156: 390–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aswani, S. (1998). Patterns of marine harvest effort in southwestern New Georgia, Solomon Islands: Resource management or optimal forging? Ocean and Coastal Management 40: 207–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnosky, A. D., Koch, P. L., Fernec, R. S., Wing, S. L., and Shabel, A. B. (2004). Assessing the causes of Late Pleistocene extinctions on the continents. Science 306: 70–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M., and Kacelnik, A. (1996). Rate currencies and the foraging starling: The fallacy of the averages revisited. Behavioral Ecology 7: 341–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M., Healy, S. D., and Hurly, T. A. (2003). Context-dependent foraging decisions in rufous hummingbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270: 1272–1276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayham, F. E. (1979). Factors influencing the Archaic pattern of animal utilization. Kiva 44: 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayham, F. E. (1982). A Diachronic Analysis of Prehistoric Animal Exploitation at Ventana Cave, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Arizona State University, Tempe.

  • Beck, C., Taylor, A., Jones, G. T., Fadem, C., Cook, C., and Millward, S. (2002). Rocks are heavy: Transport costs and Paleoarchaic quarry behavior in the Great Basin. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21: 481–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berec, L. (2000). Mixed encounters, limited perception and optimal foraging. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 62: 849–868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, C. M., Fryxell, J. M., Gates, C. C., and Fortin, D. (2001). Ungulate foraging strategies: Energy maximizing or time minimizing? Journal of Animal Ecology 70: 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1991). Hunter-Gatherers: Archaeological and Evolutionary Theory, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1993). Doing Great Basin archaeology recently: Coping with variability. Journal of Archaeological Research 1: 43–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1999). What happened in the Medithermal? In Beck, C. (ed.), Models for the Millennium: Great Basin Anthropology Today, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 62–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., and Baumhoff, M. A. (1982). The Numic spread: Great Basin cultures in competition. American Antiquity 47: 485–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., and Baumhoff, M. A. (1983). Return rates and intensity of resource use in Numic and Prenumic adaptive strategies. American Antiquity 48: 830–834.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1978). Nunamiut Ethnoarchaeology, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1980). Willow smoke and dog’s tails: Hunter-gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site formation. American Antiquity 45: 4–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1984). Faunal Remains From Klasies River Mouth, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1988). Fact and fiction about the Zinjanthropus floor: Data, arguments and interpretations. Current Anthropology 29: 123–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., and Bliege Bird, R. (1997). Contemporary shellfish gathering strategies among the Merriam of the Torres Strait Islands, Australia: Testing predictions of a central place foraging model. Journal of Archaeological Science 24: 39–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., and Bliege Bird, R. (2000). The ethnoarchaeology of juvenile foragers: Shellfishing strategies among Meriam children. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 461–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., Bleige Bird, R., and Parker, C. (2005). Aboriginal burning regimes and hunting strategies in Australia’s western desert. Human Ecology 33: 443–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., and Bird, D. W. (1997). Delayed reciprocity and tolerated theft: The behavioral ecology of food sharing strategies. Current Anthropology 38: 49–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., and Smith, E. A. (2005). Signaling theory, strategic interaction and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology 46: 221–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. (2005). What makes a competent adult forager? In Hewlett, B., and Lamb, M. (eds.), Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods, Aldine, Hawthorne, NY, pp. 109–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boone, J. L., and Smith, E. A. (1998). Is it evolution yet? A critique of evolutionary archaeology. Current Anthropology 39S: 141–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2000). Optimizing offspring: The quantity-quality tradeoff in agropastoral Kipsigis. Evolution and Human Behavior 21: 391–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botkin, S. (1980). Effects of human exploitation on shellfish populations on Malibu Creek, California. In Earle, T. K., and Christenson, A. L. (eds.), Modeling Change in Prehistoric Subsistence Economies, Academic Press, New York, pp. 121–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowman, D. M. J. S. (1998). The impact of aboriginal landscape burning on Australian biota. New Phytologist 140: 385–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brannan, J. A. (1992). On modeling resource transport costs: Suggested refinements. Current Anthropology 33: 56–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (1994a). Late Holocene resource intensification in the Sacramento Valley, California: The vertebrate evidence. Journal of Archaeological Science 21: 501–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (1994b). Declines in mammalian foraging efficiency during the late Holocene, San Francisco Bay, California. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 13: 371–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (1997). Widening diet breadth, declining foraging efficiency, and prehistoric harvest pressure: Ichthyofaunal evidence from the Emeryville Shellmound, California. Antiquity 71: 845–862.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (1999). Resource Depression and Intensification During the Late Holocene, San Francisco Bay, University of California Press, Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (2002). Prey spatial structure and behavior affect archaeological tests of optimal foraging models: Examples from the Emeryville Shellmound vertebrate fauna. World Archaeology 34: 60–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., and Bayham, F. (2003). Showing off, foraging models, and the ascendance of large-game hunting in California Middle Archaic. American Antiquity 68: 783–789.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., and Grayson, D. K. (1993). Diet breadth, adaptive change, and the White Mountain faunas. Journal of Archaeological Science 20: 331–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., and O’Connell, J. F. (1999). On evolutionary ecology, selectionist archaeology and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity 64: 153–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burger, O., Hamilton, M., and Walker, R. (2005). The prey as patch model: Optimal handling of resources with diminishing returns. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 1147–1158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burney, D. A., Robinson, G. S., and Burney, L. P. (2003). Sporormiella and the late Holocene extinctions in Madagascar. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 10800–10805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burstein, R., Coward, A. W., Askew, W. E., Carmel, K., Irving, C., Shpilberg, O., Morna, D., Pikarsky, A., Ginot, G., Sawyer, M., Golan, R., and Epstein, Y. (1996). Energy expenditure variations in soldiers performing military activities under cold and hot climate conditions. Military Medicine 161: 750–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, R. K., and Koch, P. L. (1999). Isotopic tracking of foraging and long-distance migration in northeastern Pacific pinnipeds. Oecologica 119: 578–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, R. K., Snodgrass, J. J., Gifford-Gonzalez, D., Guilderson, T., Brown, T., and Koch, P. L. (2001). Holocene changes in the ecology of northern fur seals: Insights from stable isotopes and archaeofauna. Oecologia 128: 107–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, R. K., Gifford-Gonzalez, D., Snodgrass, J. J., and Koch, P. L. (2002). Isotopic tracking of prehistoric pinniped foraging and distribution along the central California coast: Preliminary results. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 12: 4–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, V. L. (2000). Resource depression on the Northwest Coast of North America. Antiquity 74: 649–661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, V. L. (2001). Changing fish use on Mangaia, Southern Cook Islands: Resource depression and the prey choice model. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 11: 88–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, V. L., and Campbell, S. K. (2004). Resource intensification and resource depression in the Pacific Northwest of North America: A zooarchaeological review. Journal of World Prehistory 18: 327–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byers, D. A., and Broughton, J. M. (2004). Holocene environmental change, artiodactyl abundances, and human hunting strategies in the Great Basin. American Antiquity 69: 235–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byers, D. A., and Ugan, A. (2005). Should we expect large game specialization in the Late Pleistocene? An optimal foraging perspective on early Paleoindian prey choice. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 1624–1640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byers, D. A., Smith, C., and Broughton, J. M. (2005). Holocene artiodactyls population histories and large game hunting in the Wyoming Basin, USA. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 125–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, M. D. (1999). A mathematical model of the effects of screen size on zooarchaeological relative abundance indices. Journal of Archaeological Science 26: 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, M. D. (2000). Large mammal relative abundances in Pithouse and Pueblo period archaeofaunas from southwestern New Mexico: Resource depression among the Mimbres-Mogollon? Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 317–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, M. D. (2001). Archaeofaunal relative abundance, sample size, and statistical methods. Journal of Archaeological Science 28: 185–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, M. (2003). A model of central place forager prey choice and an application to faunal remains from the Mimbres Valley, New Mexico. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 22: 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caraco, T., Martindale, S., and Whittam, T. S. (1980). An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behavior 28: 820–830.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardillo, M., Mace, G. M., Jones, K. E., Beilby, J., Bininda-Emonds, O. R. P., Sechrest, W., Orme, C. D. L., and Purvis, A. (2005). Multiple causes of high extinction risk in large mammal species. Science 309: 1239–1241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. A., and Irons, W. (1979). Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective, Duxberry Press, North Scituate, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. (1976). Optimal foraging theory: The marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9: 129–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E., Orians, G., and Hyatt, K. (1976). Ecological implications of resource depression. The American Naturalist 110: 247–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. (1979). Pacific coast foragers: Affluent or overcrowded? In Koyama, S., and Thomas, D. H. (eds.), Affluent Foragers: Pacific Coasts East and West, Senri Ethnological Studies No. 9, Osaka, Japan, pp. 275–295.

  • Colten, R. H. (1995). Faunal exploitation during the middle to late period on Santa Cruz Island, California. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 17: 93–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colten, R. H., and Arnold, J. E. (1998). Prehistoric marine mammal hunting on California’s northern Channel Islands. American Antiquity 63: 679–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In Dupré, J. (ed.), The Latest on the Best: Essays on Evolution and Optimality, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 277–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw, G., Mendelson, S., and Rowcliffe, J. M. (2005). Evidence for post-depletion sustainability in a mature bushmeat market. Journal of Applied Ecology 42: 460–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (1991). Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology 20: 25–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L., Chagnon, N., and Irons, W. (eds.) (2000). Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective, Aldine de Gruyter, New York.

  • Cruz-Uribe, K. (1988). The use and meaning of species diversity and richness in archaeological faunas. Journal of Archaeological Science 15: 179–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuthill, I., and Kacelnik, A. (1990). Central place foraging: A reappraisal of the “loading effect.”. Animal Behavior 40: 1087–1101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dukas, R. (ed.) (1998). Cognitive Ecology: The Evolutionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

  • Duncan, R. P., Blackburn, T. M., and Worthy, T. H. (2002). Prehistoric bird extinctions and human hunting. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 269: 517–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dwyer, P. D. (1982). Prey switching: A case study from New Guinea. Journal of Animal Ecology 51: 529–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dwyer, P. D. (1986). Hunt in New Guinea: Some difficulties for optimal foraging theory. Man 20: 243–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egeland, C., and Byerly, C. (2005). Applications of return rates to large mammal butchery and transport among hunter-gatherers and its implications for Plio-Pleistocene hominid carcass foraging and site use. Journal of Taphonomy 3: 135–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, P. F. (1988). Foraging behavior of a central place forager: Field tests of theoretical predictions. American Naturalist 131: 159–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, J. (1966). The role of time and energy in food preference. American Naturalist 100: 611–617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etnier, M. A. (2002). The Effects of Human Hunting on Northern Fur Seal (Callorhinus ursinus) Migration and Breeding Distributions in the Late Holocene, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle.

  • Fancher, J. M., Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (2003). A comparison of duiker processing from two contemporary forager camps in the Congo Basin. Paper presented at the 68th Annual Meeting of the Society for American Archaeology, Milwaukee, WI.

  • Fiedel, S., and Haynes, G. (2004). A premature burial: Comments on Grayson and Meltzer’s “Requiem for overkill.”. Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 121–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fryxell, J. M., and Doucet, C. M. (1991). Provisioning time and central-place foraging in beavers. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadeinne de Zoologie 69: 1308–1313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gifford-Gonzalez, D. (1998). Early pastoralists in East Africa: Ecological and social dimensions. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 17: 166–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L. A., and Caraco, T. (2000). Social Foraging Theory, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, R. A. (1996). Faunal reductions at Puntutjarpa Rockshelter, Warbuton Ranges, Western Australia. Archaeology of Oceania 31: 72–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (1984). Quantitative Zooarchaeology, Academic Press, Orlando, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (1988). Danger Cave, Last Supper Cave, and Hanging Rock Shelter: The Faunas, Anthropological Papers Vol. 66, Pt. 1, American Museum of Natural History, New York.

  • Grayson, D. K. (1989). Bone transport, bone destruction, and reverse utility curves. Journal of Archaeological Science 16: 643–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (1991). Alpine faunas form the White Mountains, California: Adaptive change in the Late Prehistoric Great Basin? Journal of Archaeological Science 18: 483–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (2000). Mammalian responses to Middle-Holocene climatic change in the Greta Basin of the western United States. Journal of Biogeography 27: 181–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (2001). The archaeological record of human impacts on animal populations. Journal of World Prehistory 15: 1–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K. (2006). Holocene bison in the Great Basin, western USA. The Holocene 16: 913–925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Cannon, M. D. (1999). Human paleoecology and foraging theory in the Great Basin. In Beck, C. (ed.), Models for the Millennium: Great Basin Anthropology Today, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 141–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Delpech, F. (1998). Changing diet breadth in the early Upper Paleolithic of southwestern France. Journal of Archaeological Science 25: 119-1129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Delpech, F. (2005). Pleistocene reindeer and global warming. Conservation Biology 19: 557–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Meltzer, D. J. (2002). Clovis hunting and large mammal extinction: A critical review of the evidence. Journal of World Prehistory 14: 313–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Meltzer, D. J. (2003). A requiem for North American overkill. Journal of Archaeological Science 28: 585–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., and Meltzer, D. J. (2004). North American overkill continued? Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 133–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson, D. K., Delpech, F., Rigaurd, J. P., and Simek, J. F. (2001). Explaining the development of dietary dominance by a single ungulate taxon at Grotte XVI, Dordogne France. Journal of Archaeological Science 28: 115–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guerra, R. F., and Ades, C. (2002). An analysis of travel costs on transport of load and nest building in golden hamster. Behavioural Processes 57: 7–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilday, J. E. (1967). Differential extinctions during late-Pleistocene and recent time. In Martin, P. S., and Wright, H. E., Jr. (eds.), Pleistocene Extinctions: The Search for a Cause, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT, pp. 121–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilday, J. E. (1984). Pleistocene extinction and environmental change: A case study of the Appalachians. In Martin, P. S., and Klein, R. G. (eds.), Quaternary Extinctions: A Prehistoric Revolution, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 250–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R. B. (1987). Game conservation or efficient hunting? In McCay, B., and Acheson, J. (eds.), The Question of the Commons, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 97-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R. B. (1989). Time, efficiency and fitness in the Amazonian protein quest. Research in Economic Anthropology 11: 43–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R. B. (1991). Wildlife conservation in tribal societies. In Oldfield, M., and Alcorn, J. (eds.), Biodiversity: Culture, Conservation and Ecodevelopment, Westview Press, Boulder, CO, pp. 172–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R. B., and Vickers, W. T. (1982). Optimal diet breadth theory as a model to explain variability in Amazonian hunting. American Ethnologist 9: 358–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1990). Why do men hunt? Benefits for risky choices. In Cashdan, E. (ed.), Risk and Uncertainty in Tribal and Peasant Economies, Westview Press, Boulder, CO, pp. 145–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of a hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology 12: 29–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., and Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology 11: 58–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., and O’Connell, J. F. (1981). Affluent hunters? Some comments in light of the Alywara case. American Anthropologist 83: 622–626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., and O’Connell, J. F. (1985). Optimal foraging models and the case of the !Kung. American Anthropologist 87: 401–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., and O’Connell, J. F. (1992). On optimal foraging models and subsistence transitions. Current Anthropology 33: 63–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., Hill, K., and O’Connell, J. F. (1982). Why hunters gather-optimal foraging and the Ache of eastern Paraguay. American Ethnologist 9: 379–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., and Blurton Jones, N. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: Big game, common goods, foraging goals and the evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 334: 242–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., and Blurton Jones, N. G. (1995). Hadza children’s foraging: Juvenile dependency, social arrangements and mobility among hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology 36: 688–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, G. (2002). The catastrophic extinction of North American mammoths and mastodonts. World Archaeology 33: 391–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, G., and Eislet, B. S. (1999). The power of Pleistocene hunter-gatherers: Forward and backward searching for evidence about mammoth extinction. In MacPhee, R. D. E. (ed.), Extinctions in Near Time: Causes, Contexts, and Consequences, Kluwer/Academic, New York, pp. 71–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heglund, N., Willems, P. A., Penta, M., and Cavagna, G. (1995). Energy-saving gait mechanics with head supported loads. Nature 375: 52–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hickerson, H. (1965). The Virginia deer and intertribal buffer zones on the Upper Mississippi Valley. In Leeds, A., and Vayda, A. P. (eds.), Man’s Culture and Animals, Publication 78, American Association for the Advancement of Science, Washington, DC, pp. 43–66.

  • Hildebrandt, W. R., and Jones, T. L. (1992). Evolution of marine mammal hunting: A view from the California and Oregon coasts. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 11: 360–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrandt, W. R., and McGuire, K. (2002). Ascendence of prestige hunting during the California middle Archaic: An evolutionary perspective. American Antiquity 67: 231–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrandt, W. R., and McGuire, K. (2003). Large-game hunting, gender-differentiated work organization, and the role of evolutionary ecology in California and Great Basin prehistory: A reply to Broughton and Bayham. American Antiquity 68: 790–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (1988). Macronutrient modifications of optimal foraging theory: An approach using indifference curves applied to some modern foragers. Human Ecology 16: 157–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., and Hawkes, K. (1983). Neotropical hunting among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. In Hames, R., and Vickers, W. (eds.), Adaptive Responses of Native Amazonians, Academic Press, New York, pp. 139–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., and Hurtado, M. A. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People, Aldine de Gruyter, Hawthorne, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hawkes, K., and Hurtado, A. M. (1987). Foraging decisions among Ache hunter-gatherers: New data and implications for optimal foraging models. Ethology and Sociobiology 8: 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., Padwe, J., Bejyvagi, C., Bepurangi, A., Jakugi, F., Tykuarnangi, R., and Tykuarangi, T. (1997). Impacts of hunting on large vertebrates in the Mbaracayu Reserve, Paraguay. Conservation Biology 11: 1339–1353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., McMillan, G., and Fariña, R. (2003). Hunting-related changes in game encounter rates from 1994 to 2001 in the Mbaracayu Reserve, Paraguay. Conservation Biology 17: 1312–1323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitchcock, C. L., and Houston, A. I. (1993). The value of a hoard: Not just energy. Behavioral Ecology 5: 202–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, B. (2005). Middle and late Holocene hunting in the Great Basin: A critical review of the debate and future prospects. American Antiquity 70: 713–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houston, A. I. (1997). Natural selection and context-dependent values. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 264: 1539–1541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houston, A. I., and McNamara, J. M. (eds.) (1999). Models of Adaptive Behaviour, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • Houston, A. I., Clark, C. W., McNamara, J. M., and Mangel, M. (1988). Dynamic models in behavioral and evolutionary ecology. Nature 332: 29–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunn, E. S. (1982). Mobility as a factor limiting resource use in the Columbia Plateau of North America. In Williams, N. M., and Hunn, E. S. (eds.), Resource Managers: North American and Australian Hunter-Gatherers, Westview Press, Boulder, CO, pp. 17–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurtado, A. M., Hawkes, K., Hill, K., and Kaplan, H. (1985). Female subsistence strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers of eastern Paraguay. Human Ecology 13: 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, N. J. B., and Cowlishaw, G. (2004). How species respond to multiple extinction threats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271: 1135–1141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, T. P. (2001). Factors influencing food collection behavior for Brants’ whistling rat (Partomoys brantsii): A central place forager. Journal of Zoology 255: 15–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerozolimski, A., and Peres, C. A. (2003). Bringing home the biggest bacon: A cross-site analysis of the structure of hunter-kill profiles in Neotropical forests. Biological Conservation 111: 415–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jochim, M. (1988). Optimal foraging and the division of labor. American Anthropologist 90: 130–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. N., and Wroe, S. (2003). Causes of extinction of vertebrates during the Holocene of mainland Australia: Arrival of the dingo, or human impact? Holocene 13: 941–948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. L. (2004). Dietary evenness, prey choice, and human-environment interactions. Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 307–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. L. (2006). Prey choice, mass collecting, and the wild European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 25: 275–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, K., and Madsen, D. (1989). Calculating the cost of resource transportation: A Great Basin example. Current Anthropology 30: 529–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, K., and Metcalfe, D. (1988). Bare bones archaeology: Bone marrow indices and efficiency. Journal of Archaeological Science 15: 415–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. L., and Hildebrandt, W. R. (1995). Reasserting a prehistoric tragedy of the Commons: A reply to Lyman. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 14: 78–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. L., Hildebrandt, W. R., Kennett, D. J., and Porcasi, J. F. (2004). Prehistoric marine mammal overkill in the northeastern Pacific: A review of new evidence. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 24: 69–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A., and Bateson, M. (1996). Risky theories: The effects of variance on foraging decisions. American Zoologist 36: 402–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A., and Krebs, J. R. (1997). Yanomamo dreams and starling payloads: The logic of optimality. In Betzig, L. (ed.), Human Nature: A Critical Reader, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 21–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaspari, M. (1991). Prey preparation as a way that grasshopper sparrows (Ammodramus savannarum) increase the nutrient concentration of their prey. Behavioral Ecology 2: 234–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, C. E. (1994). Aboriginal overkill: The role of Native Americans in structuring western ecosystems. Human Nature 5: 359–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. (1995). The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways, Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. (2000). Elements of behavioral ecological paradigm for the study of prehistoric hunter-gatherers. In Schiffer, M. B. (ed.), Social Theory in Archaeology, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimball, V. (2004). The effects of multiple high-ranked prey species on the use of evenness as a proxy measure for diet breadth: An example from the southeastern Columbia Plateau. Journal of Northwest Anthropology 38: 195–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, P. V. (1996). Late Holocene human-induced modifications to a central Polynesian island ecosystem. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93: 5296–5300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapik, J., Harman, E., and Reynolds, K. (1996). Load carriage using packs: A review of physiological, biomechanical and medical aspects. Ergonomics 27: 207–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, P. A. (2004). Burden transport: When, how and how much? Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 249–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N. B. (1997). Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, 4th ed., Blackwell Science, Malden, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., and Brown, G. R. (2002). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laliberte, A. S., and Ripple, W. J. (2003). Wildlife encounters by Lewis and Clark: A spatial analysis of interactions between native Americans and wildlife. Bioscience 53: 994–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach, F., and Davidson, J. (2001). The use of size-frequency diagrams to characterize prehistoric fish catches and to assess human impact on inshore fisheries. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 11: 150–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legg, S. J., and Mahanty, A. (1984). Comparison of five methods of carrying load close to the trunk. Ergonomics 28: 1653–1660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindström, S. (1996). Great Basin fisherfolks: Optimal diet breadth modeling the Truckee River aboriginal subsistence fishery. In Plew, M. (ed.), Prehistoric Hunter-Gatherer Fishing Strategies, Boise State University Printing Services, Boise, ID, pp. 114–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Losey, R. J. (2005). Earthquakes and tsunami as elements of environmental disturbance on the northwest coast of North America. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24: 101–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D. (1995). Hadza bone assemblages and hyena attrition: An ethnographic example of the influence of cooking and mode of discard on the intensity of scavenger ravaging. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 14: 288–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D. (1998). Experimentally derived extraction rates for marrow: Implications for body part exploitation strategies of Plio-Pleistocene hominid scavengers. Journal of Archaeological Science 25: 657–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D. (2001). On the archaeological resolution of body part transport patterns: An ethnoarchaeological example from East African hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 20: 361–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D. (2006). What explains the carcass field processing and transport decisions of contemporary hunter-gatherers? Measures of economic anatomy and zooarchaeological skeletal part representation. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 13: 19-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., and O’Connell, J. F. (2002). Cut and tooth mark distributions on large animal bones: Ethnoarchaeological data from Hadza and their implications for current ideas about early human carnivory. Journal of Archaeological Science 29: 85–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (1997a). On Late Holocene variability in bison populations in the northeastern Great Basin. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 19: 50–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (1997b). Experiments in bone boiling nutritional returns and archaeological reflections. Anthropozoologica 25–26: 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (2002). Upper Paleolithic net-hunting, small prey exploitation and women’s work effort: A view from the ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological record of the Congo Basin. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 9: 147–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (2004). Meat-sharing and the archaeological record: A test of the show-off hypothesis among central African Bofi foragers. In Crothers, G. M. (ed.), Hunters and Gatherers in Theory and Archaeology, Occasional Paper No. 31, Center for Archaeological Investigations, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, pp. 241–260.

  • Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (2005). Small prey hunting technology and zooarchaeological measures of taxonomic diversity and abundance: Ethnoarchaeological evidence from Central African forest foragers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24: 335–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L. (1995). On the evolution of marine mammal hunting on the west coast of North America. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 14: 45–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L. (2003). Pinniped behavior, foraging theory, and the depression of metapopulations and nondepression of a local population on the southern Northwest Coast of North America. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 22: 376–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L. (2004). Prehistoric biogeography, abundance, and phenotypic plasticity of elk. In Lyman, R. L., and Cannon, K. (eds.), Zooarchaeology and Conservation Biology, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 136–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L. (2005). Analyzing cut marks: Lessons from artiodactyls remains in the northwestern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 1722–1732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., and O’Brien, M. J. (1998). The goals of evolutionary archaeology: History and explanation. Current Anthropology 39: 615–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., and Wolverton, S. (2002). The late prehistoric-early historic game sink in northwestern United States. Conservation Biology 16: 73–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H. (1972). Geographic Ecology: Patterns of the Distribution of the Species, Harper and Row, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H., and Pianka, E. R. (1966). On optimal use of a patchy environment. American Naturalist 100: 603–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., and Eardley, J. (2004). Maternal nutrition and sex ratio at birth in Ethiopia. Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 295–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacPhee, R. D. E., and Marx, P. A. (1997). The 40,000 year plague: Humans, hyperdisease, and first-contact extinctions. In Goodman, S. M., and Patterson, B. D. (eds.), Natural Change and Human Impact in Madagascar, Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, pp. 168–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madrigal, T. C., and Holt Zimmerman, J. Z. (2002). White-tailed deer meat and marrow return rates and their application to eastern woodlands archaeology. American Antiquity 67: 745–759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, D. B., and Kirkman, J. E. (1988). Hunting hoppers. American Antiquity 53: 593-604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, D. B., and Schmitt, D. N. (1998). Mass-collecting and the diet breadth model: A Great Basin example. Journal of Archaeological Science 25: 445–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, D. B., Scott, R. S., and Loosle, B. (2000). Differential transport costs and high altitude occupational patterns in the Unita Mountains, northeastern Utah. In Madsen, D. B., and Metcalf, M. D. (eds.), Intermountain Archaeology, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maloiy, G. M. O., Heglund, N. C., Prager, L. M., Cavagna, G. A., and Taylor, C. R. (1986). Energetic costs of carrying loads: Have African women discovered an economic way? Nature 319: 668–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mannino, M., and Thomas, K. (2002). Depletion of a resource? The impact of prehistoric human foraging on intertidal mollusk communities and its significance for human settlement, mobility and dispersal. World Archaeology 33: 452–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2004). What explains Hadza food sharing? Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 69–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2005). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Human Nature 15: 364–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. S. (1967). Prehistoric overkill. In Martin, P. S., and Wright, H. E. (eds.), Pleistocene Extinctions: The Search for a Cause, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT, pp. 75–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. F. (1983). Optimal foraging theory: A review of some models and their applications. American Anthropologist 85: 612–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. S. (1984). Prehistoric overkill: The global model. In Martin, P. S., and Klein, R. G. (eds.), Quaternary Extinctions: A Prehistoric Revolution, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 354–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. S. (1990). Who or what destroyed our mammoths? In Agenbroad, L. D., Mead, J. I., and Nelson, L. W. (eds.), Megafauna and Man: Discovery of Americas Heartland, Scientific Papers 1, Mammoth Hot Springs, ND, pp. 109–117.

  • Martin, P. S., and Szuter, C. (1999). War zones and game sinks in Lewis and Clark’s West. Conservation Biology 13: 36–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, K., and Hildebrandt, W. R. (1994). Possibilities of women and men: Gender and the California milling stone horizon. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 16: 41–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, K., and Hildebrandt, W. R. (2005). Re-thinking Great Basin foragers: Prestige hunting and costly signaling during the Middle Archaic period. American Antiquity 70: 695–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, D., and Barlow, R. K. (1992). A model for exploring the optimal trade-off between field processing and transport. American Anthropologist 94: 340–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, D., and Jones, K. (1988). A reconsideration of animal body-part utility indices. American Antiquity 53: 486–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittermeier, R. A. (1987). Effects of hunting on rain forest primates. In Marsh, C. W., and Mittermeier, R. A. (eds.), Primate Conservation in the Tropical Forest, Alan R. Liss, New York, pp. 109–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mudambo, K. S. M. T., Scrimgeour, C. M., and Rennie, M. J. (1997). Adequacy of food rations in soldiers during exercise in hot, day-time conditions assessed by doubly labeled water and energy balance methods. European Journal of Applied and Occupational Physiology 76: 346–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munro, N. D. (2004). Zooarchaeological measures of hunting pressure and occupational intensity in the Natufian: Implications for agricultural origins. Current Anthropology 45: S5–S34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munro, N. D., and Bar-Oz, G. (2005). Gazelle bone fat processing in the Levantine Epipaleolithic. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagaoka, L. (2001). Using diversity indices to measure changes in prey choice at the Shag River Mouth site, New Zealand. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 11: 101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagaoka, L. (2002a). The effects of resource depression on foraging efficiency, diet breadth, and patch use in southern New Zealand. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21: 419–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagaoka, L. (2002b). Explaining subsistence change in southern New Zealand using foraging theory models. World Archaeology 34: 84–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noss, A. J. (1995). Duikers, Cables, and Nets: Cultural Ecology of Hunting in a Central African Forest, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Florida, Gainesville.

  • Nagaoka L. (2005). Declining foraging efficiency and mao carcass exploitation in southern New Zealand. Journal of Archaeological Science 32: 1328–1338.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., and Leonard, R. D. (1998). Basic incompatibilities between evolutionary and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity 63: 485–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F. (1995). Ethnoarchaeology needs a general theory of behavior. Journal of Archaeological Research 3: 205–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., and Hawkes, K. (1984). Food choice and foraging sites among the Alyawara. Journal of Anthropological Research 40: 504–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Blurton Jones, N. (1988). Hadza hunting, butchering, and bone transport and their archaeological implications. Journal of Anthropological Research 44: 113–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Blurton Jones, N. (1990). Reanalysis of large mammal body part transport among the Hadza. Journal of Archaeological Science 17: 301–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H., and Pearson, N. (1979). On the theory of central place foraging. In Horn, D. J., Stairs, G. R., and Mitchell, R. D. (eds.), Analysis of Ecological Systems, Ohio State University Press, Columbus, pp. 155–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peacock, E., Haag, W. R., and Warren, M. L., Jr. (2004). Prehistoric decline in freshwater mussels coincident with the advent of maize agriculture. Conservation Biology 19: 547–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. J. (2003). The evolutionary roots of our environmental problems: Towards a Darwinian ecology. Quarterly Review of Biology 78: 275–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, J. (2004). Resource intensification and environmental variability: Subsistence patterns in middle and late period deposits at CA-SBA-2225, Vandenberg Air Force Base, California. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 24: 81–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimm, S. L. (1991). The Balance of Nature? Ecological Issues in the Conservation of Species and Communities, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinson, A. O. (1999). Foraging in Uncertain Times: The Effects of Risk on Subsistence Behavior During the Pleistocene-Holocene Transition in the Oregon Great Basin, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque.

  • Porcasi, J. F., and Fujita, H. (2000). The dolphin hunters: A specialized prehistoric maritime adaptation in the Southern California Channel Islands and Baja California. American Antiquity 65: 543–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porcasi, J. F., Jones, T. L., and Raab, L. M. (2000). Trans-Holocene marine mammal exploitation on San Clemente Island, California: A tragedy of the commons revisited. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 200–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, J. M. (1995). The effects of sedentism on the processing of hunted carcasses in the southwest: A comparison of two Pueblo IV sites in central New Mexico. Kiva 60: 411–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pullam, H. (1974). On the theory of optimal diets. American Naturalist 108: 59–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyke, G. (1984). Optimal foraging theory: A critical review. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15: 523–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quirt-Booth, T., and Cruz-Uribe, K. (1997). Analysis of leporid remains from prehistoric Sinagua sites, northern Arizona. Journal of Archaeological Science 24: 945–960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raab, L. M. (1992). Optimal foraging analysis of prehistoric shellfish collecting on San Clemente Island, California. Journal of Ethnobiology 12: 63–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasheed, S. A., and Harder, L. D. (1997). Foraging currencies for non-energetic resources: Pollen collection by bumblebees. Animal Behavior 54: 911–926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raymond, A. W., and Sobel, E. (1990). The use of Tui chub as food by Indians of the western Great Basin. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology 12: 2–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redford, K., and Robinson, J. G. (1987). The game of choice: Patterns of Indian and colonist hunting in the Neotropics. American Anthropologist 89: 650–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhode, D. (1990). On transportation costs of Great Basin resources: An assessment of the Jones-Madsen model. Current Anthropology 31: 413–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, M. P., Pettit, P. B., Stiner, M. C., and Trinkaus, E. (2001). Stable isotopic evidence for increasing dietary breadth in the European mid-Upper Paleolithic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 811: 6528–6532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saint-Germain, C. (1997). The production of bone broth: A study in nutritional exploitation. Anthropozoologica 25–26: 153–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salant, S. W., Kalat, K. L., and Wheatcroft, A. M. (1995). Deducing implications of fitness maximization when a trade-off exists among alternate currencies. Behavioral Ecology 6: 424–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sassaman, K. (2004). Complex hunter-gatherers in evolution and history: A North American perspective. Journal of Archaeological Research 12: 227–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1996). Some relationships between behavioral and evolutionary archaeologies. American Antiquity 61: 643–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, K. (1998). The consequences of partially directed search effort. Evolutionary Ecology 12: 263–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. N., and Lupo, K. D. (1995). On mammalian taphonomy, taxonomic diversity, and measuring subsistence data in zooarchaeology. American Antiquity 60: 496–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. N., and Lupo, K. D. (2005). The Camels Back Cave mammalian fauna. In Schmitt, D. N., and Madsen, D. B. (eds.), Camels Back Cave, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 136–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. N., Madsen, D. B., and Lupo, K. D. (2004). The worst of times, the best of times: Jackrabbit hunting by Middle Holocene human foragers in the Bonneville Basin of western North America. In Mondini, M., Muñoz, S., and Wickler, S. (eds.), Colonisation, Migration and Marginal Areas: A Zooarchaeological Approach, Oxbow Books, Oxford, pp. 86–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoener, T. W. (1979). Generality of the size-distance relation in models of optimal foraging. American Naturalist 114: 902–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simenstad, C. A., Estes, J. A., and Kenyon, K. W. (1978). Aleuts, sea otters, and alternate stable-state communities. Science 100: 403–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simms, S. R. (1987). Behavioral Ecology and Hunter-Gatherer Foraging: An Example from the Great Basin, BAR International Series 381, British Archaeological Reports, Oxford.

  • Smith, E. A. (1979). Human adaptation and energetic efficiency. Human Ecology 7: 53–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (1983). Anthropological applications of optimal foraging theory: A critical review. Current Anthropology 24: 625–651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (1991). Inujjuamiut Foraging Strategies: Evolutionary Ecology of an Arctic Hunting Economy, Aldine de Gruyter, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2000). Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In Cronk, L., Chagnon, N., and Irons, W. (eds.), Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective, Aldine de Gruyter, New York, pp. 27–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature 15: 343–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., and Bliege Bird, R. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening: Public generosity as costly signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior 21: 245–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., and Winterhalder, B. (eds.) (1992). Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior, Aldine de Gruyter, New York.

  • Smith, E. A., and Wishnie, M. (2000). Conservation and subsistence in small-scale societies. Annual Review of Anthropology 29: 493–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., and Hill, K. (2001). Controversies in the evolutionary social sciences: A guide for the perplexed. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 16: 128–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodhi, N. S. (1992). Central place foraging and prey preparation by a specialist predator, the merlin. Journal of Field Ornithology 63: 71–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2000). Costly signaling and torch fishing on Ifaluk atoll. Evolution and Human Behavior 21: 223–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2002). Patch choice decisions among Ifaluk fishers. American Anthropologist 104: 583–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. D. (1983). Bison Kills and Bone Counts, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. D. (1991). Some unexplored aspects of mutualistic Plains-Pueblo food exchange. In Spielmann, K. (ed.), Farmers, Hunters, and Colonists: Interaction Between the Southwest and Southern Plains, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 18–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. D., and Scott, S. L. (1989). Horticulture and large-mammal hunting: The role of resource depletion and the constraints of time and labor. In Kent, S. (ed.), Farmers as Hunters: The Implications of Sedentism, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 71–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. D., and Spielmann, K. (1983). Energy source, protein metabolism, and hunter-gatherer subsistence strategies. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 2: 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinney, L. (1995). Women with a head for weights. New Scientist 146: 4062–4079.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, P. (1996). Holocene biodiversity: An archaeological perspective from the Americas. Annual Review of Anthropology 25: 105–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steadman, D. W. (1995). Prehistoric extinctions of Pacific island birds: Biodiversity meets zooarcheology. Science 267: 1123–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stearman, A. M. (1994). Only savages climb trees-revisiting the myth of the ecologically noble savage in Amazonia. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 5: 339–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, D. W., and Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C. (2001). Thirty years on the “broad spectrum revolution” and Paleolithic demography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 6993–6996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C., and Munro, N. D. (2002). Approaches to prehistoric diet breadth , demography and prey ranking systems in time and space. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 9: 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C., Munro, N. D., Surovell, T. A., Tchernov, E., and Bar-Yosef, O. (1999). Paleolithic population growth pulses evidenced by small animal exploitation. Science 283: 190–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C., Munro, N. D., and Surovell, T. (2000). The tortoise and the hare: Small-game use, the broad spectrum revolution, and Paleolithic demography. Current Anthropology 41: 39–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surovell, T. A. (2000). Early Paleoindian women, children and mobility. American Antiquity 65: 493–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surovell, T. A., Waguespack, N., and Brantingham, P. J. (2005). Global archaeological evidence for proboscidean overkill. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 6231–6236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1987). If we’re all Darwinians, what’s the fuss about? In Crawford, C., Smith, M., and Krebs, D. (eds.), Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues and Applications, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 121–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1989). A critique of Darwinian anthropology. Ethology and Sociobiology 10: 131–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1990). Adaptiveness and adaptations. Ethology and Sociobiology 11: 427–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szuter, C. R., and Bayham, F. E. (1989). Sedentism and prehistoric animal procurement among desert horticulturists of the North American southwest. In Kent, S. (ed.), Farmers as Hunters: The Implications of Sedentism, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 80–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology 11: 375–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, F. (2002). An evaluation of central-place foraging among mollusk gatherers in Western Kiribati, Micronesia: Linking behavioral ecology with ethnoarchaeology. World Archaeology 34: 187–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ugan, A. (2005a). Does size matter? Body size, mass collecting, and their implications for understanding prehistoric foraging behavior. American Antiquity 70: 75–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ugan, A. (2005b). Climate, bone density, and resource depression: What is driving variation in large and small game in Fremont archaeofaunas? Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24: 227–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ugan, A., and Bright, J. (2001). Measuring foraging efficiency with archaeological faunas: the relationship between relative abundance indices and foraging returns. Journal of Archaeological Science 28: 1309–1321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ugan, A., Bright, J., and Rogers, A. (2003). When is technology worth the trouble? Journal of Archaeological Science 30: 1315–1329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, W. T. (1988). Game depletion hypothesis of Amazonian adaptation data from a native community. Science 239: 1521–1522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, W. T. (1991). Hunting yields and game composition over ten years in an Amazon Indian Territory. In Robinson, J. G., and Redford, K. H. (eds.), Neotropical Wildlife Use and Conservation, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 53–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, W. T. (1994). From opportunism to nascent conservation: The case Siona-Secoya. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspectives 5: 307–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voland, E. (1998). Evolutionary ecology of human reproduction. Annual Review of Anthropology 27: 347–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waguespack, N. M., and Surovell, T. A. (2003). Clovis hunting strategies, or how to make out on plentiful resources. American Antiquity 68: 333–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waite, T. A., and Ydenberg, R. C. (1994). What currency do scatter-hoarding gray jays maximize? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 34: 43–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., and McMillan, G. (2002). Age-dependency in hunting ability among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Journal of Human Evolution 42: 639–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weimerskirch, H., Ancel, A., Calion, M., Zaharley, A., Spagiari, J., Kersten, M., and Chastel, O. (2003). Foraging efficiency and adjustment of energy expenditure in a pelagic sea bird provisioning its chick. Journal of Animal Ecology 72: 500–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welham, C. V. J., and Beauchamp, G. (1997). Parental provisioning in a variable environment: Evaluation of three foraging currencies and a state variable model. Evolutionary Ecology 11: 399–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, T. (1952). Observations of butchering techniques of some aboriginal peoples: No. 1. American Antiquity 17: 337–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, T. (1953). Observations of butchering techniques of some aboriginal peoples: No. 2. American Antiquity 19: 160–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, T. (1954). Observations of butchering techniques of some aboriginal peoples: Nos. 3, 4, 5 and 6. American Antiquity 19: 254–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (2002). Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange: A long term perspective on !Kung (Ju/’hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior 23: 407–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1981). Foraging strategies in the Boreal Forest: An analysis of Cree hunting and gathering. In Winterhalder, B., and Smith, E. A. (eds.), Hunter-Gatherer Foraging Strategies: Ethnographic and Archeological Analyses, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 66–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1986). Optimal foraging: Simulation studies of diet choice in a stochastic environment. Journal of Ethnobiology 6: 206–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., and Lu, F. (1997). A forager-resource population ecology model and implications for indigenous conservation. Conservation Biology 11: 1354–1364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., and Smith, E. A. (eds.) (1981). Hunter-Gatherer Foraging Strategies: Ethnographic and Archeological Analysis, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

  • Winterhalder, B., and Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 51–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., Flora, L., and Tucker, B. (1999). Risk-sensitive adaptive tactics: Models and evidence from subsistence studies in biology and anthropology. Journal of Archaeological Research 7: 301–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlgemuth, E. (2004). Archaeobotanical remains. In McGuire, K., Delacorte, M., and Carpenter, K. (eds.), Archaeological Excavations at Pie Creek and Tule Valley Shelter, Elko County, Nevada, Anthropological Papers No. 25, Nevada State Museum, Carson City, pp. 96–103.

  • Wolverton, S. (2005). The effects of the hypsithermal on prehistoric foraging efficiency in Missouri. American Antiquity 70: 91–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ydenberg, R. C. (1998). Behavioral decisions about foraging and predator avoidance. In Dukas, R. (ed.), Cognitive Ecology: The Evolutionary Ecology of information Processing and Decision Making, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 343–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ydenberg, R. C., and Hurd, P. (1998). Simple models of feeding with time and energy constraints. Behavioral Ecology 9: 49–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ydenberg, R. C., Welham, C. V. J., Schmid-Hempel, R., Schmid-Hempel, P., and Beauchamp, G. (1992). Time and energy constraints and the relationships between currencies in foraging theory. Behavioral Ecology 5: 28–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yesner, D. (1981). Archaeological applications of optimal foraging theory: Harvest strategies of Aleut hunter-gatherers. In Winterhalder, B., and Smith, E. A. (eds.), Hunter-Gatherer Foraging Strategies: Ethnographic and Archeological Analysis, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 148–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yesner, D. (1994). Seasonality and resource “stress” among hunter-gatherers: Archaeological signatures. In Burch, E. S., Jr., and Ellana, L. J. (eds.), Key Issues in Hunter-Gatherer Research, Berg, Providence, RI, pp. 151–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection-a selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1977). Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism. In Stonehouse, B., and Perrins, C. M. (eds.), Evolutionary Ecology, Macmillan Press, London, pp. 252–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeanah, D. W. (2004). Sexual division of labor and central place foraging: A model for the Carson Desert of western Nevada. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 23: 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeanah, D. W., and Simms, S. R. (1999). Modeling the gastic: Great Basin subsistence studies since 1982 and the evolution of general theory. In Beck, C. (ed.), Models for the Millennium: Great Basin Anthropology Today, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 118–141.

    Google Scholar 

Bibliography of selected recent literature (post-1990)

  • Alvard, M. S., and Kuznar, L. (2001). Deferred harvests: The transition from hunting to animal husbandry. American Anthropologist 103: 295–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, K. R., and Metcalfe, D. (1996). Plant utility indices: Two Great Basin examples. Journal of Archaeological Science 23: 351–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.) (1997). Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Papers No. 7, American Anthropological Association, Arlington, VA.

  • Beaton, J. M. (1991). Extensification and intensification in central California prehistory. Antiquity 65: 947–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begossi, A. (1992). Use of optimal foraging theory in the understanding of fishing strategies: A case from Sepetiba Bay (Rio de Janeiro State, Brazil). Human Ecology 20: 463–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, I. M. (1991). Bari loricarid collection and the value of information: An application of optimal foraging theory. Human Ecology 19: 517–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., Malhi, R., and McCarthy, H. (1997). Central place models of acorn and mussel processing. Journal of Archaeological Science 24: 887–900.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betts, M. W., and Friesen, T. M. (2004). Quantifying hunter-gatherer intensification: A zooarchaeological case study from Arctic Canada. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 23: 357–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betts, M. W., and Friesen, T. M. (2006). Declining foraging returns from an inexhaustible resource? Abundance indices and beluga whaling in western Canadian Arctic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 25: 59–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boone, J. L. (2002). Subsistence strategies and early human population history: An evolutionary ecological perspective. World Archaeology 34: 6–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bright, J., Ugan, A., and Hunsaker, L. (2002). The effect of handling time on subsistence technology. World Archaeology 34: 164–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brink, J. (1997). Fat content in leg bones of Bison bison and applications to archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Science 24: 259–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, D. A., Josephson, S. C., and Coltrain, J. B. (1994). Burkina Faso herdsmen and optimal foraging theory: A reconsideration. Human Ecology 22: 213–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W. (2004). Privatization, small-seed intensification, and the origin of pottery in the western Great Basin. American Antiquity 69: 653–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elston, R. G., and Zeanah, D. W. (2002). Thinking outside the box: A new perspective on diet breadth and sexual division of labor in the Prearchaic Great Basin. World Archaeology 34: 103–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, H. T. (2003). Dynamic optimization of horticulture among the Muscogee Creek Indians of the southeastern United States. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 22: 411–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gremillion, K. (2002). Foraging theory and hypothesis testing in archaeology: An exploration of methodological problems and solutions. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21: 142–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Hill, K., and Jakugi, F. (2004). Why do forager share and sharers forage? Explorations of the social dimensions of foraging. Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 19–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993). Why hunter-gatherers work: An ancient version of the problem of public goods. Current Anthropology 34: 341–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1996). The evolutionary basis of sex variations in the use of natural resources: Human examples. Population and Environment 18: 161–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrikson, S. L. (2004). Frozen bison and fur trapper’s journals: Building a prey choice model for Idaho’s Snake River Plain. Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 90–916.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilton, C. E., and Greaves, R. D. (2004). Age, sex, and resource transport in Venezuelan foragers. In Meldrum, D. J., and Hilton, C. (eds.), From Biped to Strider: The Emergence of Modern Human Walking. Running and Resource Transport, Kluwer/Plenum, New York, pp. 163–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janetski, J. C. (1997). Fremont hunting and resource intensification in the eastern Great Basin. Journal of Archaeological Science 24: 1075–1089.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, K., and Madsen, D. B. (1991). Further experiments in native food procurement. Utah Archaeology 4: 68–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. L. (2001). Prehistory of the Carson Desert and Stillwater Mountains: Environment, Mobility, and Subsistence in a Great Basin Wetland, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, F. E. (2001). The common property regime of the Huaorini Indians of Ecuador: Implications and challenges to conservation. Human Ecology 29: 425–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, D. B., and Schmitt, D. N. (eds.) (2005). Buzz-Cut Dune and Fremont Foraging at the Margin of Horticulture, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.

  • Marean, C. W., and Cleghorn, N. (2003). Large mammal skeletal element transport: Applying foraging theory in a complex taphonomic system. Journal of Taphonomy 1: 15–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Blurton Jones, N. G., and Lupo, K. D. (2002). Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. Journal of Human Evolution 43: 831–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborn, A. J. (1993). Snowblind in the desert southeast: Moisture islands, ungulate ecology, and alternative prehistoric overwintering strategies. Journal of Anthropological Research 49: 135–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Outram, A. K. (2004). Identifying dietary stress in marginal environments: Bone fats, optimal foraging theory and the seasonal round. In Mondini, M., Muñoz, S., and Wickler, S. (eds.), Colonization, Migration and Marginal Areas: A Zooarchaeological Approach, Oxbow Books, Oxford, pp. 74–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raab, L. M., and Jones, T. (eds.) (2004). Prehistoric California: Archaeology and the Myth of Paradise, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.

  • Schmitt, D. N., Madsen, D. B., and Lupo, K. D. (2002). Small-mammal data on early and middle Holocene climates and biotic communities in the Bonneville Basin, USA. Quaternary Research 58: 255–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, B. (2004). Giving, scrounging, hiding, and selling: Minimal food sharing among Mikae of Madagascar. Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 45–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, E. (2001). The sustainability of resources used by Native Americans on four Caribbean Islands. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 11: 112–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (2002). Behavioral and other human ecologies: Critique, response and progress through criticism. Journal of Ecological Anthropology 6: 4–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeanah, D. W. (2002). Central place foraging and prehistoric pinyon utilization in the Great Basin. In Fitzhugh, B., and Habu, J. (eds.), Beyond Foraging and Collecting: Evolutionary Change in Hunter-Gatherer Settlement Systems, Kluwer/Plenum, New York, pp. 231–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeleznik, W. S., and Bennett, I. M. (1991). Assumption validity in human optimal foraging-the Bari hunters of Venezuela as a test case. Human Ecology 19: 499–508.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank Dave Schmitt for reading and editing earlier versions of this article, producing the figures, and providing inspirational support. I thank Don Grayson for sharing his views on extinction during a rambling phone call on a rainy afternoon last year. The anonymous reviewers provided very helpful comments that greatly enhanced this article. It is impossible for me to adequately cite all of the recent anthropological research in human behavioral ecology. I therefore refer interested readers to the very complete and useful online bibliography by Kermyt Anderson found at http://faculty-staff.ou.edu/A/Kermyt.G.Anderson-1/HBE/. I thank Kermyt for allowing me to cite this bibliography.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen D. Lupo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lupo, K.D. Evolutionary Foraging Models in Zooarchaeological Analysis: Recent Applications and Future Challenges. J Archaeol Res 15, 143–189 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10814-007-9011-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10814-007-9011-1

Keywords

Navigation