Skip to main content
Log in

Joint Engagement, Parent Labels, and Language Development: Examining Everyday Interactions in Infant Siblings of Children with Autism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined joint engagement, parent labels, and language development in infants with an elevated (EL) and typical likelihood (TL) for ASD. Parent–child interactions were coded for joint engagement and parent labels at 12 and 18 months, and language skills were assessed later in toddlerhood for 12 EL infants diagnosed with ASD (EL-ASD), 17 EL infants with language delay (EL-LD), 14 EL infants with no diagnosis (EL-ND), and 12 TL infants. Infants spent substantial time in supported joint engagement and received similar rates of input from parents across outcome groups. However, parents of EL-ASD infants increased the rate of labels provided in coordinated joint engagement. While labels positively predicted language for TL infants, the opposite pattern emerged for EL-ASD infants.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. While the field has traditionally referred to EL infants as “high risk” (HR), this terminology conveys a view that having an autism spectrum diagnosis is inherently negative. While many individuals with autism need substantial support, many also view their autism as a positive aspect of their identity (e.g., Kenny et al., 2016; Robison, 2019). We use the terms “elevated likelihood” (EL) and “typical likelihood” (TL) here in place of the stigmatizing language predominant in the medical model of autism.

  2. EL infants were evaluated at 36 months with the exception of one EL infant who was evaluated at 24 months of age and received an ASD diagnosis prior to withdrawing from the study. This participant is included in analyses.

  3. Four infants (one in each outcome group) spent 0% of the observation at both 12 and 18 months in coordinated JE. An additional 8 infants (2 TL, 2 EL-ND, 0 EL-LD, 4 EL-ASD) at 12 months and 12 infants (1 TL, 2 EL-ND, 7 EL-LD, 2 EL-ASD) at 18 months spent 0% of the observation in coordinated JE.

  4. For both the 12-month and 18-month regressions, analyses were performed with and without controlling for concurrent expressive language. The pattern of results for each model was the same, with two exceptions: when concurrent language was not included for 18-month labels in the overall observation and for 18-month labels in coordinated JE, the EL-ND interaction terms were also significant (ps < 0.05). Additionally, post-hoc analyses rotating the reference group with and without concurrent language were equivalent unless otherwise specified, and results are presented throughout with concurrent expressive language in the model.

References

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., & Deckner, D. F. (2004). The development of symbol-infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75(4), 1171–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F., & Romski, M. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 84–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L.B., Bakeman, R., Russell, C.L., Deckner, D.F. (1998). Coding symbol-infused engagement states. (Technical Report 9). Georgia State University.

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Suma, K., & Robins, D. L. (2019). An expanded view of joint attention: Skill, engagement, and language in typical development and autism. Child Development, 90(1), e1–e18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., Kaiser, A. P., Tamis-LaMonda, C. S., Owen, M. T., & Dimitrova, N. (2020). The developmental landscape of early parent-focused language intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 59–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders5.

  • Baio, J., Wiggins, L., Christensen, D. L., Maenner, M. J., Daniels, J., Warren, Z., … Durkin, M. S. (2018). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 67(6), 1.

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 14, 1278–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. Joint Attention, 131, 158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bani Hani, H., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. S. (2013). Children’s referential understanding of novel words and parent labeling behaviors: Similarities across children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 971–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergelson, E., Amatuni, A., Dailey, S., Koorathota, S., & Tor, S. (2019). Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. L., Guthrie, W., Coffing, M., & Lord, C. (2011). Convergent validity of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning and the differential ability scales in children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116(5), 331–343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bottema-Beutel, K., Kim, S. Y., Crowley, S., Augustine, A., Kecili-Kaysili, B., Feldman, J., & Woynaroski, T. (2019). The stability of joint engagement states in infant siblings of children with and without ASD: Implications for measurement practices. Autism Research, 12(3), 495–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bottema-Beutel, K., & Kim, S. Y. (2021). A systematic literature review of autism research on caregiver talk. Autism Research, 14(3), 432–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bottema-Beutel, K., Lloyd, B., Watson, L., & Yoder, P. (2018). Bidirectional influences of caregiver utterances and supported joint engagement in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 11(5), 755–765.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bottema-Beutel, K., Yoder, P. J., Hochman, J. M., & Watson, L. R. (2014). The role of supported joint engagement and parent utterances in language and social communication development in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(9), 2162–2174.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brugman, H., Russel, A. (2004). Annotating Multimedia/ Multi-modal resources with ELAN. In: Proceedings of LREC 2004, Fourth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation.

  • Bruner, J. (1985). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 1(1), 111–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B., Mahoney, A. S., Northrup, J., Moore, E. L., Leezenbaum, N. B., & Brownell, C. A. (2018). Developmental changes in pretend Play from 22-to 34-months in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(3), 639–654.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 358(1430), 315–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Young, G. S., Brian, J., Carter, A., Carver, L. J., Chawarska, K., … Hertz-Picciotto, I. (2017). Non-ASD outcomes at 36 months in siblings at familial risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Autism Research, 10(1), 169–178.

  • Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 195–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, B., Nelson, C. A., Rowe, M. L., & Tager‐Flusberg, H. (2020). Reciprocal influences between parent input and child language skills in dyads involving high‐and low‐risk infants for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research.

  • Crandall, M. C., McDaniel, J., Watson, L. R., & Yoder, P. J. (2019). The relation between early parent verb input and later expressive verb vocabulary in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(6), 1787–1797.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Croen, L. A., Najjar, D. V., Fireman, B., & Grether, J. K. (2007). Maternal and paternal age and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 334–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delehanty, A. D., & Wetherby, A. M. (2021). Rate of communicative gestures and developmental outcomes in toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorder during a home observation. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drumm, E., & Brian, J. (2013). The developing language abilities and increased risks of’unaffected’siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiatry, 3(5), 513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., ... & Reilly, J. S. (1993). MacArthur communicative development inventories: user's guide and technical manual Singular Publishing Group. San Diego.

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., ... & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i-185.

  • Fleiss, J. L., Levin, B., & Paik, M. C. (1981). The measurement of interrater agreement. Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions, 2(212–236), 22–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franchini, M., Duku, E., Armstrong, V., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Garon, N.,… Smith, I. M. (2018). Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(10), 3417–3431.

  • Fusaroli, R., Weed, E., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2019). Hearing me hearing you: Reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development. Cognition, 183, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, J., Pickles, A., Pasco, G., Bedford, R., Wan, M. W., Elsabbagh, M., … Charman, T. (2017). Randomised trial of a parent-mediated intervention for infants at high risk for autism: longitudinal outcomes to age 3 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(12), 1330–1340.

  • Gulsrud, A. C., Hellemann, G., Shire, S., & Kasari, C. (2016). Isolating active ingredients in a parent-mediated social communication intervention for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(5), 606–613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haebig, E., McDuffie, A., & Weismer, S. E. (2013). Brief report: Parent verbal responsiveness and language development in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(9), 2218–2227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Towson: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilmann, J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J., & Hollar, C. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur—bates communicative development inventory in identifying language abilities of late-talking and typically developing toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), 40–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., … Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–1083.

  • Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–365.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iakoucheva, L. M., Muotri, A. R., & Sebat, J. (2019). Getting to the cores of autism. Cell, 178(6), 1287–1298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Wozniak, R. H. (2016). Transitions to intentional and symbolic communication in typical development and in autism spectrum disorder. In Prelinguistic and minimally verbal communicators on the autism spectrum (pp. 51–72). Springer, Singapore.

  • Iverson, J. M., Northrup, J. B., Leezenbaum, N. B., Parladé, M. V., Koterba, E. A., & West, K. L. (2018). Early gesture and vocabulary development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(1), 55–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jakubowski, K. P., & Iverson, J. M. (2019). Look at mommy: an exploratory study of attention-related communication in mothers of toddlers at risk for autism. Language Learning and Development, 15(2), 126–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaswal, V. K., & Akhtar, N. (2019). Being versus appearing socially uninterested: Challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, 1589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. J., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A. C., Wong, C., Kwon, S., & Locke, J. (2010). Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1045–1056.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20(4), 442–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leezenbaum, N. B., Campbell, S. B., Butler, D., & Iverson, J. M. (2014). Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism, 18(6), 694–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longard, J., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Duku, E., Moore, C., Smith, I. M., … Bryson, S. (2017). Early expressive and receptive language trajectories in high-risk infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 2, 2396941517737418.

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., … Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

  • Maenner, M. J., Shaw, K. A., & Baio, J. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 69(4), 1.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Marrus, N., Hall, L. P., Paterson, S. J., Elison, J. T., Wolff, J. J., Swanson, M. R., … Zwaigenbaum, L. (2018). Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 10(1), 29.

  • Manwaring, S. S., Stevens, A. L., Mowdood, A., & Lackey, M. (2018). A scoping review of deictic gesture use in toddlers with or at-risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 2396941517751891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDuffie, A. S., Yoder, P. J., & Stone, W. L. (2006). Labels increase attention to novel objects in children with autism and comprehension-matched children with typical development. Autism, 10(3), 288–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Smith, I., & Bryson, S. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S69–S78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (pp. 58–64). Circle Pines, MN: AGS.

  • Northrup, J. B., & Iverson, J. M. (2015). Vocal coordination during early parent–infant interactions predicts language outcome in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Infancy, 20(5), 523–547.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., … Steinfeld, M. B. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266.

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Hutman, T. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495.

  • Patterson, S. Y., Elder, L., Gulsrud, A., & Kasari, C. (2014). The association between parental interaction style and children’s joint engagement in families with toddlers with autism. Autism, 18(5), 511–518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickles, A., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., Salomone, E., Cole-Fletcher, R., Tobin, H., … Aldred, C. (2016). Parent-mediated social communication therapy for young children with autism (PACT): Long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 388(10059), 2501–2509.

  • Robins, D. L., Fein, D., & Barton, M. (2009). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers, revised with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F). Pediatrics, 133, 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roemer, E. J. (in press). Beyond the toddler years: A meta-analysis of communicative abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Bulletin.

  • Robison, J. E. (2019). Talking about autism—thoughts for researchers. Autism Research, 12(7), 1004–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language., 10, 381–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., Leech, K. A., & Cabrera, N. (2017). Going beyond input quantity: Wh-questions matter for toddlers’ language and cognitive development. Cognitive Science, 41, 162–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., Dapretto, M., & Sigman, M. (2011). Behavioral profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: Contribution of measures of mother–infant interaction and nonverbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 287–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. (2009). The transactional model. American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., Hutman, T., & Sigman, M. (2013). A parent-mediated intervention to increase responsive parental behaviors and child communication in children with ASD: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 540–555.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suarez-Rivera, C., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2019). Multimodal parent behaviors within joint attention support sustained attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 55(1), 96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, M., Finelli, J., Marvin, A., Garrett-Mayer, E., Bauman, M., & Landa, R. (2007). Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorder: A prospective study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 37–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, M. R. (2020). The role of caregiver speech in supporting language development in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 1230–1239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, M. R., Donovan, K., Paterson, S., Wolff, J. J., Parish-Morris, J., Meera, S. S., … Shen, M. D. (2019). Early language exposure supports later language skills in infants with and without autism. Autism Research, 12(12), 1784–1795.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2016). Risk factors associated with language in autism spectrum disorder: Clues to underlying mechanisms. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(1), 143–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Talbott, M. R., Nelson, C. A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2016). Maternal vocal feedback to 9-month-old infant siblings of children with ASD. Autism Research, 9(4), 460–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences, 10(1), 69–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 18, 1454–1463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trautman, C. H., & Rollins, P. R. (2006). Child-centered behaviors of caregivers with 12-month-old infants: Associations with passive joint engagement and later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(3), 447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigil, D. C., Hodges, J., & Klee, T. (2005). Quantity and quality of parental language input to late-talking toddlers during play. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21(2), 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Readings on the Development of Children, 23(3), 34–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wan, M. W., Green, J., Elsabbagh, M., Johnson, M., Charman, T., Plummer, F., & BASIS Team. (2012). Parent–infant interaction in infant siblings at risk of autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(3), 924–932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wan, M. W., Green, J., Elsabbagh, M., Johnson, M., Charman, T., Plummer, F., & Basis Team. (2013). Quality of interaction between at-risk infants and caregiver at 12–15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 763–771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Joint attention without gaze following: Human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e79659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2016). Multiple sensory-motor pathways lead to coordinated visual attention. Cognitive Science., 8, 418–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., Suanda, S. H., & Smith, L. B. (2019). Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD054979 and R01 HD073255) to JMI. Additional support for recruitment was provided by UL1 RR024153 and UL1 TR000005 to the University of Pittsburgh Clinical and Translational Science Institute. We would like to thank Alyssa Cooper, Amanda Shields, Sydney Tan, and Alyson Menzies for their help with behavioral coding and members of the Infant Communication Lab for assistance with data collection. Special thanks to Celia Brownell and Jennifer Silk for their insightful comments and to the infants and families who participated in this research. This research was completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science at the University of Pittsburgh. Portions of these data were presented at the 2018 International Congress of Infant Studies, Philadelphia, PA, the 2018 Joint International Workshop “Understanding and Promoting Change from Early to Complex Skills in Typical and Atypical Development”, Bologna, Italy, and the 2021 Society for Research in Child Development virtual meeting.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EJR jointly conceived of and designed the study, completed behavioral coding, conducted analyses and interpretation of data, and drafted the manuscript. EHK contributed to interpretation of data analyses, assisted with figures, and provided revisions and feedback on the manuscript. JMI jointly conceived of and designed the study, coordinated data collection, assisted in interpretation of data analyses, and provided revisions and feedback on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emily J. Roemer.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained for all individual participants in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Roemer, E.J., Kushner, E.H. & Iverson, J.M. Joint Engagement, Parent Labels, and Language Development: Examining Everyday Interactions in Infant Siblings of Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 52, 1984–2003 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05099-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05099-1

Keywords

Navigation