Skip to main content
Log in

Do Implicit and Explicit Racial Biases Influence Autism Identification and Stigma? An Implicit Association Test Study

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Are implicit and explicit biases related to ASD identification and/or stigma? College students (N = 493) completed two IATs assessing implicit stigma and racial biases. They evaluated vignettes depicting a child with ASD or conduct disorder (CD) paired with a photo of a Black or White child. CD was more implicitly and explicitly stigmatized than ASD. Accurately identifying ASD was associated with reduced explicit stigma; identifying CD led to more stigma. Participants who identified as White implicitly associated the White child with ASD and the Black child with CD. A trend in the reverse direction was observed among Black participants. Implicit and explicit biases were unrelated. Findings highlight a need for trainings to ameliorate biases favoring one’s in-group.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We use identity-first language (e.g., autistic people) rather than person-first language (e.g., people with autism) because identity-first language is preferred by autistic people (Kapp et al.; 2013; Kenny et al., 2016) and may be less likely to contribute to stigma (Gernsbacher, 2017). As recommended by Dunn and Andrews (2015), the APA now recommends that researchers follow the preferences of minority groups by using the terms that they prefer to describe them (https://www.apa.org/pi/disability/resources/choosing-words).

  2. In order to be consistent with the terms used by the creators of the Child Affective Facial Expression database (CAFÉ; Lobue and Thrasher 2015), which we used to construct our racial bias IAT, we used the terms “European Americans” and “African Americans” in the Racial Bias IAT. Anonymous reviewers pointed out limitations with this choice of terms, specifically that participants had no reason to infer that a White child was necessarily of European descent or a Black child was necessarily African American. Given limitations in our choice of terms (which are discussed in more detail in the limitations section), the terms White and Black are used throughout the paper except when describing the construction of the racial bias IAT and the exact results obtained from it in the results section.

  3. Reaction time data from Stage 1 of the Disability Valence IAT was used to explore the construct validity (i.e., internal consistency) of the target phrases. If all terms characterize the category equally well (i.e., have high construct validity) there should be similarity in the average latency to categorize different targets from the same category. Cronbach’s alpha was used to compare average latencies for target term for each participant.

  4. For the improved D score algorithm, this process is completed again by subtracting participants’ average reaction time to sort target terms in Stage 7 by participants’ average reaction time to sort target terms in Stage 4.

  5. There was also a difference in reaction times when participants sorted the ASD and CD phrases between the Disability Valence and Racial Bias IAT. Participants were significantly faster when sorting the ASD and CD phrases (M = 1.28 s; SD = 0.44) in Stage 1 of the Racial Bias IAT compared to Stage 2 of the Disability Valence IAT (M = 1.58 s; SD = 0.52), t(467) = 13.35, p < 0.001. Participants also made significantly fewer errors when sorting the ASD and CD phrases (M = 0.93 errors; SD = 1.31) in Stage 4 of the Racial Bias IAT compared to Stage 4 of the Disability Valence IAT (M = 1.13 errors; SD = 1.53), t(467) = 2.55, p = .011. These differences between the Disability Valence IAT and the Racial Bias IAT were likely the result of increased familiarity with the phrases since the Racial Bias IAT was always presented second.

  6. We focus on spontaneous judgements in analyses as they were more sensitive than structured ratings of the likelihood of a given condition in prior research (Beeger et al. 2009). However, a similar pattern of findings was observed with spontaneous and structured ratings.

  7. This relationship remained significant when the analysis was run with only participants who accurately identified both ASD and CD, Z = -16.79, p < 0.001.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Atkins-Loria, S., Macdonald, H., & Mitterling, C. (2015). Young African American men and the diagnosis of conduct disorder: The neo-colonization of suffering. Clinical Social Work Journal, 43(4), 431–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Autism Self-Advocacy Network. https://autisticadvocacy.org/.

  • Baio, J. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. The Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) Surveillance Summaries 67(6):1–23

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32(7), 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(1), 53–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Begeer, S., El Bouk, S., Boussaid, W., Terwogt, M. M., & Koot, H. M. (2009). Underdiagnosis and referral bias of autism in ethnic minorities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 142–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Begeer, S., Mandell, D., Wijnker-Holmes, B., Venderbosch, S., Rem, D., Stekelenburg, F., et al. (2013). Sex differences in the timing of identification among children and adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(5), 1151–1156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, I. V., Havranek, E. P., Price, D. W., Hanratty, R., Fairclough, D. L., Farley, T., … Steiner, J. F. (2013). Assessment of biases against Latinos and African Americans among primary care providers and community members. American Journal of Public Health, 103(1), 92–98.

  • Bogardus, E. S. (1933). A social distance scale. Sociology & Social Research, 17, 265–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryson, S. E., Rogers, S. J., & Fombonne, E. (2003). Autism spectrum disorders: early detection, intervention, education, and psychopharmacological management. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48(8), 506–516.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, D. A., Koot, H. M., de Wilde, A., & Begeer, S. (2016). Influence of child factors on health-care professionals’ recognition of common childhood mental-health problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(10), 3083–3096.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. M., & Barger, B. D. (2014). Peers’ knowledge about and attitudes towards students with autism spectrum disorders. In V. B. Patel, V. R. Preedy, & C. R. Martin (Eds.), Comprehensive guide to autism (pp. 247–261). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., Pogacar, R., Pullig, C., Kouril, M., Aguilar, S., LaBouff, J., et al. (2019). Survey-software implicit association tests: A methodological and empirical analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 51(5), 2194–2208.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Columb, C., & Plant, E. A. (2016). The Obama effect six years later: The effect of exposure to Obama on implicit anti-Black evaluative bias and implicit racial stereotyping. Social Cognition, 34(6), 523–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan, P. W., Bink, A. B., Fokuo, J. K., & Schmidt, A. (2015). The public stigma of mental illness means a difference between you and me. Psychiatry Research, 226(1), 186–191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N. (2004). Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioral manifestations. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 143–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., & Rivera, L. M. (2008). When social context matters: The influence of long-term contact and short–term exposure to admired outgroup members on implicit attitudes and behavioral intentions. Social Cognition, 26(1), 112–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, R., Davis, D. W., Jones, V. F., Keating, A., & Wildman, B. (2015). The role of stigma in parental help-seeking for perceived child behavior problems in urban, low-income African American parents. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 22(4), 265–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donohue, M. R., Childs, A. W., Richards, M., & Robins, D. L. (2019). Race influences parent report of concerns about symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 23(1), 100–111.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 62–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, M. S., Maenner, M. J., Baio, J., Christensen, D., Daniels, J., Fitzgerald, R., … Wingate, M. S. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder among US children (2002–2010): Socioeconomic, racial, and ethnic disparities. American Journal of Public Health, 107(11), 1818–1826.

  • Dunn, D. S., & Andrews, E. E. (2015). Person-first and identity-first language: Developing psychologists’ cultural competence using disability language. American Psychologist, 70(3), 255–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, J. M., Andersen, L. M., & Bejerot, S. (2013). RAADS-14 Screen: validity of a screening tool for autism spectrum disorder in an adult psychiatric population. Molecular Autism, 4(1), 49.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, N. D., Morrell, H. E., & Neece, C. (2016). Predictors of age of diagnosis for children with autism spectrum disorder: The role of a consistent source of medical care, race, and condition severity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 127–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, D. B., & Crandall, C. S. (2007). Dimensions of mental illness stigma: What about mental illness causes social rejection? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26(2), 137–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2016). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 365–382.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foroni, F., & Bel-Bahar, T. (2010). Picture-IAT versus Word-IAT: level of stimulus representation influences on the IAT. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(2), 321–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freng, S., & Kehn, A. (2013). Determining true and false witnessed events: Can an eyewitness-implicit association test distinguish between the seen and unseen. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 20(5), 761–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, E., & Iarocci, G. (2014). Students with autism spectrum disorder in the university context: Peer acceptance predicts intention to volunteer. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 1008–1017.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (2017). Editorial perspective: The use of person-first language in scholarly writing may accentuate stigma. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(7), 859–861.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B. L., Rochat, P., Tone, E. B., & Baron, A. S. (2017). Sources of implicit and explicit intergroup race bias among African-American children and young adults. PLoS ONE, 12(9), e0183015.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Brooks, P. J., Someki, F., Obeid, R., Shane-Simpson, C., Kapp, S. K., ... Smith, D. S. (2015). Changing college students’ conceptions of autism: An online training to increase knowledge and decrease stigma. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2553–2566.

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Daou, N., Obeid, R., Reardon, S., Khan, S., & Goldknopf, E. (2020). What contributes to stigma towards autistic college students and students with other diagnoses? Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Daou, N., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Kapp, S. K., Obeid, R., Brooks, P. J., et al. (2019). Factors underlying cross-cultural and gender differences in stigma towards ASD: Insights from an online training for college students in Lebanon and the United States. Autism. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318823550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golijani-Moghaddam, N., Hart, A., & Dawson, D. L. (2013). The implicit relational assessment procedure: Emerging reliability and validity data. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 2(3–4), 105–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenland, S., Senn, S. J., Rothman, K. J., Carlin, J. B., Poole, C., Goodman, S. N., & Altman, D. G. (2016). Statistical tests, P values, confidence intervals, and power: A guide to misinterpretations. European Journal of Epidemiology, 31(4), 337–350.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, W., Swineford, L. B., Nottke, C., & Wetherby, A. M. (2013). Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 582–590.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. V., Phillips, K. W., & Townsend, S. S. (2015). A rose by any other name?: The consequences of subtyping “African-Americans” from “Blacks”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 183–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J. N., Daniels, A. M., Koenig, K., Palmer, K., … Taylor, J. L. (2015). Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 36.

  • Harrison, A. J., Bradshaw, L. P., Naqvi, N. C., Paff, M. L., & Campbell, J. M. (2017). Development and psychometric evaluation of the Autism Stigma and Knowledge Questionnaire (ASK-Q). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(10), 3281–3295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. J., Paff, M. L., & Kaff, M. S. (2019). Examining the psychometric properties of the autism stigma and knowledge questionnaire (ASK-Q) in multiple contexts. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 57, 28–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatzenbuehler, M. L. (2016). Structural stigma: Research evidence and implications for psychological science. American Psychologist, 71(8), 742.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, P. J., & Sears, D. O. (2002). The symbolic racism 2000 scale. Political Psychology, 23(2), 253–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. P., Zuckerman, K. E., & Fombonne, E. (2014). Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. In Volkmar, F., Rogers, S., Paul, R., & Pelphrey, K. A. (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders, 4th ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

  • Hinshaw, S. P., & Stier, A. (2008). Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 367–393.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kapp, S. K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Sherman, L. E., & Hutman, T. (2013). Deficit, difference, or both? Autism and neurodiversity. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Amodio, D. M., & Hugenberg, K. (2017). Intergroup perception and cognition: An integrative framework for understanding the causes and consequences of social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 1–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, A., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2013). Implicit attitudes towards children with autism versus normally developing children as predictors of professional burnout and psychopathology. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(1), 17–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20(4), 442–462.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harnum, M., Duffy, J., & Ferguson, D. A. (2007). Adults’ versus children’s perceptions of a child with autism or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1337–1343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota, J. T., Peiso, J., Marcum, K., & Cloutier, J. (2017). Intergroup contact throughout the lifespan modulates implicit racial biases across perceivers’ racial group. PLoS ONE, 12(7), e0180440.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, C. K., Hoffman, K. M., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). Reducing implicit prejudice. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(5), 315–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2007). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: IV: What we know (so far) about the method. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 59–102). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Link, B. G., Phelan, J. C., Bresnahan, M., Stueve, A., & Pescosolido, B. A. (1999). Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health, 89(9), 1328–1333.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Link, B. G., Yang, L. H., Phelan, J. C., & Collins, P. Y. (2004a). Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(3), 511–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, L., Stamm, K., & Christidis, P. (2018). How diverse is the psychology workforce? News from APA’s Center for Workforce Studies. Monitor on Psychology, 49(2), 19.

    Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & Thrasher, C. (2015). The Child Affective Facial Expression (CAFE) set: Validity and reliability from untrained adults. Frontiers in Psychology., 5, 1532. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01532.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2013). Labeling and stigma. In B. G. Link & J. C. Phelan (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of mental health (pp. 525–541). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magaña, S., Parish, S. L., Rose, R. A., Timberlake, M., & Swaine, J. G. (2012). Racial and ethnic disparities in quality of health care among children with autism and other developmental disabilities. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50(4), 287–299.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, D. (2008). College students’ attitudes toward individuals with autism. Dissertation Abstracts International, 68(11-B), 7672.

  • Mandell, D. S., & Palmer, R. (2005). Differences among states in the identification of autistic spectrum disorders. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 159(3), 266–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandell, D. S., Ittenbach, R. F., Levy, S. E., & Pinto-Martin, J. A. (2007). Disparities in diagnoses received prior to a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1795–1802.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandell, D. S., Listerud, J., Levy, S. E., & Pinto-Martin, J. A. (2002). Race differences in the age at diagnosis among Medicaid-eligible children with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(12), 1447–1453.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandell, D. S., Wiggins, L. D., Carpenter, L. A., Daniels, J., DiGuiseppi, C., Durkin, M. S., … Shattuck, P. T. (2009). Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Public Health, 99(3), 493–498.

  • Matthews, M. (2019). Why Sheldon Cooper can’t be black: The visual rhetoric of autism and ethnicity. Journal of Literary & Cultural Disability Studies, 13(1), 57–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mizock, L., & Harkins, D. (2011). Diagnostic bias and conduct disorder: Improving culturally sensitive diagnosis. Child & Youth Services, 32(3), 243–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/0145935X.2011.605315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. M., Nyhan, B., & Torres, M. (2018). How conditioning on posttreatment variables can ruin your experiment and what to do about it. American Journal of Political Science, 62(3), 760–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarro, D. (2013). Learning statistics with R: A tutorial for psychology students and other beginners: Version 0.5. Adelaide: University of Adelaide.

  • Nelson, A. (2002). Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparities in health care. Journal of the National Medical Association, 94(8), 666–668.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19(6), 625–666. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obeid, R., Daou, N., DeNigris, D., Shane-Simpson, C., Brooks, P. J., & Gillespie-Lynch, K. (2015). A cross-cultural comparison of knowledge and stigma associated with autism spectrum disorder among college students in Lebanon and the United States. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3520–3536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2499-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O'Shea, B., Watson, D. G., & Brown, G. D. (2016). Measuring implicit attitudes: A positive framing bias flaw in the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). Psychological Assessment, 28(2), 158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J., & Mackenzie, D. (2018). The book of why: The new science of cause and effect. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rae, J. R., Newheiser, A. K., & Olson, K. R. (2015). Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 535–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rai, D., Lewis, G., Lundberg, M., Araya, R., Svensson, A., Dalman, C., … & Magnusson, C. (2012). Parental socioeconomic status and risk of offspring autism spectrum disorders in a Swedish population-based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(5), 467–476.

  • Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 119–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the implicit association test: Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutland, A., & Killen, M. (2015). A developmental science approach to reducing prejudice and social exclusion: Intergroup processes, social–cognitive development, and moral reasoning. Social Issues and Policy Review, 9(1), 121–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruxton, G. D., & Neuhäuser, M. (2010). When should we use one-tailed hypothesis testing? Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1(2), 114–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadler, J. Z. (2014). Conduct disorder as a vice-laden diagnostic concept. In C. Perring & L. Wells (Eds.), Diagnostic dilemmas in child and adolescent psychiatry: Philosophical perspectives (pp. 166–181). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarrett, J. C. (2011). Trapped children: Popular images of children with autism in the 1960s and 2000s. Journal of Medical Humanities, 32(2), 141–153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., Faso, D. J., Nugent, J., Lovell, S., Kennedy, D. P., & Grossman, R. B. (2017). Neurotypical peers are less willing to interact with those with autism based on thin slice judgments. Scientific Reports, 7, 40700.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Assessment of individual differences in implicit cognition: A review of IAT measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24(4), 210–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segall, M. J., & Campbell, J. M. (2014). Factors influencing the educational placement of students with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(1), 31–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigelman, L., Tuch, S. A., & Martin, J. K. (2005). What’s in a name? Preference for “Black” versus “African-American” among Americans of African descent. Public Opinion Quarterly, 69(3), 429–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Someki, F., Torii, M., Brooks, P. J., Koeda, T., & Gillespie-Lynch, K. (2018). Stigma associated with autism among college students in Japan and the United States: An online training study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 76, 88–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, J. L., Stein, M. T., Olson, L. M., Frintner, M. P., & Radecki, L. (2009). Reflections on well-child care practice: A national study of pediatric clinicians. Pediatrics, 124(3), 849–857.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thibodeau, R., & Finley, J. R. (2017). On associative stigma: Implicit and explicit evaluations of a mother of a child with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(3), 843–850.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C., & Tajfel, H. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. Psychology of Intergroup Relations, 5, 7–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valicenti-McDermott, M., Hottinger, K., Seijo, R., & Shulman, L. (2012). Age at diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. The Journal of Pediatrics, 161(3), 554–556.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenger, J. L., & Yarbrough, T. D. (2005). Religious individuals: Evaluating their intrinsic and extrinsic motivations at the implicit level of awareness. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145(1), 5–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westcott, K. (2015). Race, criminalization, and historical trauma in the United States: Making the case for a new justice framework. Traumatology, 21(4), 273–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/trm0000048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. C., & Scior, K. (2014). Attitudes towards individuals with disabilities as measured by the Implicit Association Test: A literature review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(2), 294–321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. C., & Scior, K. (2015). Implicit attitudes towards people with intellectual disabilities: Their relationship with explicit attitudes, social distance, emotions and contact. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0137902.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, K. E., Mattox, K., Donelan, K., Batbayar, O., Baghaee, A., & Bethell, C. (2013). Pediatrician identification of Latino children at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics, 132(3), 445–453.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bauman, M. L., Choueiri, R., Kasari, C., Carter, A., Granpeesheh, D., … Pierce, K. (2015). Early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder under 3 years of age: Recommendations for practice and research. Pediatrics, 136(Supplement 1), S60–S81.******

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants in this study. Initial findings from this study were presented in 2019 at the Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association and the International Society for Autism Research (INSAR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RO and JBB share first authorship and contributed equally to the manuscript with KGL, the advising author. RO led survey development, supervised and collaborated with AC and FJ in data cleaning/coding, conducted analyses and a literature review and wrote drafts of this manuscript. JBB led development of the IATs and processing of the IAT data, helped develop the rest of the survey and the analytic approach and contributed to the literature review and manuscript revisions. AC helped develop pilot surveys and the final survey, conducted initial analyses and wrote an initial draft of this manuscript as her honors thesis. AC and FJ collaborated in data cleaning and coding. AJH aided with the project conceptualization, collected data from Georgia and contributed to survey development and manuscript editing. SS contributed to survey development and manuscript editing. KGL developed the initial idea for this study, supervised survey design and analytic approach, collected data in NYC, acted as AC’s advisor on the draft of this manuscript for her thesis, and contributed very substantially to the literature review and writing of this manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Rita Obeid, Jennifer Bailey Bisson or Kristen Gillespie-Lynch.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 718 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Obeid, R., Bisson, J.B., Cosenza, A. et al. Do Implicit and Explicit Racial Biases Influence Autism Identification and Stigma? An Implicit Association Test Study. J Autism Dev Disord 51, 106–128 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04507-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04507-2

Keywords

Navigation