Food selectiveness, or picky/fussy eating, can be defined as a lack of variety in the diet (Carruth et al. 1998) or as consumption of a limited number of foods (Rydell et al. 1995). In developmentally disordered (DD) populations, such as those with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) or intellectual disability (ID), rates of food selectiveness (Williams and Seiverling 2010), eating related problem behaviours (Ledford and Gast 2006) and rejection of both novel and already accepted foods (Seiverling et al. 2018) are common. Consequently, food selectivity and refusal often results in people missing, or having very low levels, of important nutrients in the diet (Esteban-Figuerola et al. 2018; Sharp et al. 2013). This may lead to micronutrient deficiencies and in turn specific health consequences, such as reduced bone growth due to low calcium intake (Hediger et al. 2008) or scurvy due to low levels of vitamin C (Ma et al. 2016). However, these effects can be prevented through habitual intake of a varied diet and specifically by increasing intake of nutrient-dense fruit and vegetables (FV). Yet, feeding interventions with people with ASD often aim to increase the volume of food consumed and rarely consider increasing the variety of foods consumed (Marshall et al. 2015b).

Ledford et al. (2018) reviewed the various interventions used to treat feeding related behaviours in the ASD population. These methods included escape extinction (EE), fading techniques and positive reinforcement, among other strategies. The techniques are predominantly used in clinical or feeding disordered populations where fussy eating has caused other problems, such as malnutrition, growth faltering or failure to thrive (Barnhill et al. 2017). Fewer studies have assessed techniques used to promote more general components of healthy eating in DD populations such as increasing variety which have far less urgent clinical need than those used to address more serious protein-energy malnutrition or micronutrient deficiency but are nonetheless valuable to ensure optimal food intake.

In typically-developing populations, rejected foods often include nutrient-dense FV that may be bitter in taste or unusual in appearance (Dovey et al. 2008). Interventions designed to increase intake of FV in typically developing populations have been based on mere exposure effects (Barends et al. 2019; Nekitsing et al. 2018). However, repeated exposures are rarely used in the same format to increase healthy eating and variety in DD populations (e.g. storybooks or classroom games; Coulthard and Ahmed 2017; Heath et al. 2014). The current review therefore aims to identify interventions used with DD populations and to assess their effectiveness in promoting healthy eating behaviours including increasing dietary variety. This review focused on measures of increased intake of novel or previously rejected foods, increased number of foods consumed, food choice and whether interventions used have lasting effects. Secondary aims were to identify the settings in which these interventions were carried out, who the interventions were implemented by, what diagnoses the participants of interventions had and the types of study design used.

Methods

Registration

The protocol for carrying out this review was specified and registered in advance with the International prospective register of systematic reviews PROSPERO, registration number: CRD42019116769.

Eligibility Criteria

The PICOS (participant, intervention, comparison, outcomes and study-design) framework was used to develop inclusion and exclusion criteria for the review. Studies were considered for inclusion if their sample consisted of participants of any age with a diagnosis of a DD. DD was used as a broad term to include cognitive or learning DD including ASD, ID, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), or other disorders of a similar nature (e.g. developmental delay, Down’s syndrome). Studies were excluded from the review if they included participants with eating disorders (avoidant-restrictive food intake disorder was also excluded), failure to thrive (or tube-dependent participants), Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, diabetes or pregnancy. Participants with other mental health conditions or unrelated medical conditions were also excluded.

Included studies were those which had employed an intervention exposing participants to food stimuli, techniques to increase food intake or environmental changes around mealtime eating. Control groups for interventions were not restricted and studies were considered for inclusion if they used pre-intervention measures of food intake as a control for post-intervention measures.

Primary outcome measures related to healthy eating were acceptable. These included: amount of food eaten (weighed) if a variety of foods were introduced, number of different foods eaten, number or percentage of bites eaten and self-reported intakes. Studies were excluded if they did not report food intake, or if the amount or types of foods used in the intervention were not reported. Any study design was considered for inclusion dependent on whether an intervention was carried out.

Studies were also excluded if (1) only problem behaviours at mealtimes (e.g. tantrums, aggressive behaviours) were reported, (2) the full text was not in English or (3) the study was published before the year 2000. It was deemed that papers before this time may not be relevant to the current review as diagnostic criteria for DD were revised in this year (American Psychiatric Association 2000).

Information Sources and Search Strategy

Published articles were identified through searching electronic databases and scanning reference lists of previous, similar reviews. Limits were placed on language (English) and year of publication (01/01/2000-07/11/2018). This search was applied to five databases, Ovid (MEDLINE 1996–present; EMBASE 1996–present and PsychInfo 2002–present), EBSCO (CINAHL 1960–present) and Web of Science (core collection 1900–present). All searches were conducted on 07/11/2018. A full list of search terms based on the PICOS criteria are provided in Table 1 and were used to search all databases. Search terms for each relevant PICOS criterion were adapted from search terms used by previous systematic reviews (Brown et al. 2016; Brylewski and Duggan 1999; Sharp et al. 2017; Veltman et al. 2005; Williams et al. 2006).

Table 1 Search strategy of OVID (Medline, Embase and PsychInfo), EBSCO (CINAHL) and Web of Science (Core collection) based on PICOS criteria

Study Selection

Two thousand six hundred and sixteen studies were identified using the search strategy. Retrieved studies were initially searched for duplicates which left a remaining 1668 papers to be screened. Titles and abstracts were screened in full by one reviewer and 5% by two separate reviewers (2.5% each; n = 84) who were blinded to the first reviewers’ decisions. Of these double-screened papers, five were identified for further screening and agreement was 100% between reviewers. Fifty-seven papers in total were identified for full text screening. At this stage, all papers eligible were screened in full by one reviewer and 50% each by two independent reviewers. The decision of each reviewer was blinded from other reviewers, but the studies being screened were not blinded to author or journal information. Disagreements between reviewers were resolved by consensus and agreement rate was 83%. In total, 21 papers were removed due to not meeting the eligibility criteria, leaving 36 studies to be included in the review (PRISMA flow diagram; Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow chart of the study selection procedure

Data Collection Process

A data extraction sheet (Cochrane Public Health Group Data Extraction and Assessment Template 2011) was modified to suit the needs of the current review. This was tested on five papers at the full text screening stage and refined accordingly. One reviewer extracted data for 100% of the studies and this was checked by two other reviewers. Disagreements were resolved by discussion between two reviewers and if necessary, it was planned that the third reviewer would have final say on resolving disputes.

Data Items

Extracted information from each included study was based on the relevant PICOS criteria. This included key characteristics of the study (country of study and setting of the study), study design, participants involved (age, gender and DD diagnoses), characteristics of the intervention (strategies used, focus of the intervention and duration) and study outcomes (number of foods eaten, percentage of bites accepted, measured intake).

Risk of Bias

Risk of bias for individual studies was assessed using the Single-Case Design-Risk of Bias tool (SCD-RoB) developed by Reichow et al. (2018). This tool was chosen as it allowed the reviewers to evaluate the validity of the findings in single-case research designs and compare the study quality with other research designs. The tool assesses each study for selection bias (sequence generation and participant selection), performance bias (blinding of participants and personnel and procedural fidelity) and detection bias (blinding of outcome assessors, selective outcome reporting, dependent variable reliability and data sampling) (Reichow et al. 2018). For each of these criteria, the risk is reported as low, high or unclear.

Summary Measures

Primary outcome measures included number of foods eaten or change in the variety of foods eaten (especially number of novel foods eaten reported from different food groups), actual food intake (measured in either grams or bites eaten), percentage of food accepted from food offered and self-reported food intake (food diaries).

Synthesis of Results

The outcomes of included studies were synthesised to identify key characteristics of interventions used (study design, duration, setting and techniques used in intervention) and the effectiveness of these interventions to increase healthy eating in DD populations. The studies were grouped based on the intervention used and outcome measures reported. It was decided that quantitative analyses, in the form of a meta-analysis, was not appropriate due to the types of study design that were included in the review and the large variance in the interventions implemented in the studies.

Results

Study Selection

The PRISMA flow diagram (Fig. 1) provides an overview of study selection with reasons for exclusion. Searches of Medline, Embase, Psychinfo, CINAHL and Web of Science returned 2616 results and after duplicates and any papers before the year 2000 were removed, 1668 references remained. Of these, 1611 were not relevant to the current review and 57 were full text screened. A further 21 papers were excluded, including three that did not have participants with DD and a further three studies with tube dependent participants. Three studies did not report the number of foods used, six did not report food intake and one study did not implement an intervention. A further two studies were excluded for being unpublished dissertations or theses and the full text for three papers were not accessible online and the authors were not contactable. A total of 36 studies were subsequently identified to be included in the review.

Study Characteristics

Characteristics of included studies are summarised in Table 2.

Table 2 Study characteristics and healthy eating outcomes for included intervention studies

Methods

Thirty of the 36 included studies were case studies or case series. Three used pre–post intervention designs, one study used a cross sectional intervention versus control design, one examined a retrospective chart review and one study was a parallel-group randomized clinical trial. 30 studies were conducted in the USA, with one study each conducted in the UK, Canada, Sweden, Australia, South Korea and Japan.

Participants

A total of 317 participants took part across the 36 included studies, with the majority being male (n = 217). As age was not conditional, the studies included participants with an age range of 2 to 22 years. Most studies included children between 2 and 8 years, although a few studies included age ranges of 16–22 years. Participants were also mostly diagnosed with ASD, with a few studies including participants with a diagnosis of ID, pervasive developmental delay, global and specific developmental delays, Down’s syndrome and ADHD.

Intervention

Thirty-two studies were evaluated on pre-intervention and post-intervention measures, and four studies included either a control group or a parallel intervention group. Studies rarely used one intervention alone and most interventions were implemented as a package with multiple strategies used. Techniques used in interventions are described in Table 3.

Table 3 A description of the intervention techniques used and how many studies in the current review used the technique

Setting

Nineteen studies were carried out in clinical or laboratory settings, nine were set in the home and a further five studies took place in school settings (see Table 2). The remaining three studies did not report where interventions took place.

Outcomes

All studies collected and reported outcomes related to food intake and 19 studies reported these values for baseline, post-intervention and at follow-up time points. Measures of food intake included total number of foods eaten, number of pieces of foods eaten (pieces were sometimes defined as 1.5 cm3 of food; Cassey et al. 2016), percentage of accepted (consumed) bites during a meal, reported intake from food diaries, measured weight of food consumed (grams) and behavioural demands, such as picking up food, touching the lips and eventually swallowing the food.

Risk of Bias Within Studies

Table 4 outlines the risk of bias judgements of the reviewer for the 36 included studies. For the current review, some of these criteria were not applicable to individual study designs. Using the SCD-RoB tool, case studies included in the review were generally rated as low-risk of bias, whereas other study designs were rated as having a higher risk of bias.

Table 4 Assessment of study quality using the SCD-RoB tool (Color table online)

Synthesis of Results

A narrative synthesis of included studies was carried out. This details the common themes that occur and clusters studies based on the interventions used and their outcome measures.

Theory Driven Interventions

Interestingly, 34 of the 36 studies retrieved reported having positive or effective results. All studies except three were grounded in learning theory to shape eating behaviour. Two studies not based on learning theory used environmental changes grounded in behavioural economic strategies (Hubbard et al. 2015; Wallen et al. 2013), neither of which were effective. The third delivered psychoeducation to parents (Miyajima et al. 2017). This study was effective at increasing number of foods eaten (a mean increase of five foods).

Learning theory, based on the principles of classical and operant conditioning, was the most common theme throughout the interventions used. In total, 21 studies included a behaviour reinforcement component that was central to the intervention, with many more studies involving positive reinforcement as usual practice after a target behaviour was achieved. Of these, 14 studies used DRA (differential reinforcement of alternative behaviour) and nine used NCR (non-contingent reinforcement) procedures to reinforce target behaviours during eating. Procedures included a range of personalised reinforcers, including access to preferred toys (Binnendyk and Lucyshyn 2009) and preferred foods (Seiverling et al. 2012a), to reward the target behaviour (eating a novel or non-preferred food). Both techniques were effective when used as part of an intervention and some interventions used DRA and NCR techniques together (e.g. Seiverling et al. 2012a).

Eighteen studies used EE (escape extinction) techniques to prevent participants from avoiding the target food and seven of these EE studies employed NRS (non-removal of the spoon). EE procedures were reported to be effective for increasing acceptance of bites in all studies that used the technique (Allison et al. 2012). In contrast to this method, exposure, systematic desensitisation and various fading techniques (texture, portion, and demand) were used in 15 studies as a means of gradually exposing the participant to more of the food stimulus, or different textures of the same food stimulus. These techniques were grounded in learning theory in the same way as EE, however they were based on gradual introduction to the novel stimulus rather than a flooding experience of the food stimulus. Increasing exposure was also effective at increasing intake of new and disliked foods, although exposure on its own may be less effective than EE to increase eating of new foods in selective eaters (Kim et al. 2018).

Four studies employed scaffolding techniques to prompt appropriate responses from participants. This included physical (e.g. hand over hand) and verbal prompts, encouraging the participant to carry out the target behaviour themselves by showing them what to do (Wood et al. 2009). Similarly, two studies employed social learning theory in the form of modelling eating as part of an intervention package, which was reported as having limited effectiveness (Fu et al. 2015).

Single Versus Multiple Component Interventions

The majority of studies combined components during interventions, with only two studies purely using single-component interventions to increase eating outcomes. Patel et al. (2007) and Ewry and Fryling (2016) both reported that high-probability instructional sequences increased behavioural outcomes and percentage of bites accepted when compared to low-probability instructional sequences. All other 34 studies used multiple-components in their interventions, although five studies phased their approach to implementing interventions and therefore illustrate that some interventions were not effective when presented alone. Fu et al. (2015) found that modelling did not increase intake from 0%, but when DRA was implemented together with modelling, intake increased to 70% and finally 100% when NRS was also introduced. Similarly, Kadey et al. (2013) reported that NRS and NCR procedures together did not increase acceptance of novel foods for one participant, but these outcomes increased when physical guidance was added.

EE and NRS techniques were generally effective for increasing consumption of bites offered and these techniques were usually added when other strategies did not work. For example, Najdowski et al. (2003) illustrated that DRA and demand fading did not have effective outcomes until EE was added to the intervention. Similarly, Penrod et al. (2010) showed that adding EE to the intervention resulted in swallowing new foods for two participants. For the third participant, DRA, escape and bite fading already resulted in some acceptance, so when reinforcer manipulation was added, intake increased further and EE was not needed. These reports suggest the need for a hierarchy of interventions to apply when particular techniques do not achieve a meaningful increase in acceptance in selective eaters. It appears that when reinforcement or systematic desensitisation techniques are not effective, EE and NRS can be effective at increasing intake especially when the participant is extremely resistant to other methods. However, if this does not work, physical guidance may lead to positive outcomes such as accepting a non-preferred food, as illustrated by Kadey et al. (2013).

Micro Versus Macro Outcomes

Reported outcomes for each study also varied, with 24 studies reporting percentage of food accepted or number of bites consumed and 17 studies reporting total number of foods eaten and the number of new or disliked foods that were eaten before and after intervention. There were also differences within outcome measures reported. Total number of foods eaten was sometimes reported as number of food items and at others as number of pieces of that food item (e.g. how many chips were eaten; Brown et al. 2002). The number of new foods eaten ranged from + 2 to + 63 foods, although the majority of interventions reported between +5 and +15 foods. This is in contrast to the percentage of bites eaten from 0 to 100% of a single new food.

Interestingly, many different interventions, including graduated exposure, DRA, fading techniques and EE, were all successful for increasing the number of new foods eaten (Marshall et al. 2015a; Paul et al. 2007; Tanner and Andreone 2015), whereas for increasing intake, DRA, EE, NCR, high probability sequences and physical prompts were all successful at increasing percentage of bites accepted (Allison et al. 2012; Pizzo et al. 2012; VanDalen and Penrod 2010). This illustrates that although different outcomes were measured, similar techniques were used whether the target was on a macro (whole new foods eaten) or micro scale (bites of a new food presented). Exposure techniques were more commonly used when attempting to increase the number of new foods eaten (Barahona et al. 2013; Brown et al. 2002) and EE techniques were employed in most effective interventions increasing acceptance of bites (Fernand et al. 2016; Najdowski et al. 2010). Nevertheless, both techniques were successful for both outcomes.

Duration of Intervention

Duration of interventions were reported either in time or number of sessions, although some studies did not report this clearly or did not state the intensity of delivered sessions (Cosbey and Muldoon 2017). Studies lasted from 15 sessions (Cassey et al. 2016) to 129 sessions (Penrod et al. 2010), or from 5-days (Seiverling et al. 2012) to a minimum of 6-months (Wallen et al. 2013). Other studies set goals before termination of the intervention, such as until 15 new foods were accepted or until 22-weeks had elapsed (Koegel et al. 2012).

Furthermore, only 19/36 studies had a follow-up. This ranged from weekly at 2, 4, 6 and 12-weeks (Najdowski et al. 2003, 2010) to the longest follow-ups at 1-year (VanDalen and Penrod 2010) and 2-years (Binnendyk and Lucyshyn 2009). However, 17 studies did not have (or did not report) a follow-up time-point to evaluate the long-term effects of the studies. Of the studies that did follow-up, there was some loss of performance, but outcomes were generally better than baseline. Binnendyk and Lucyshyn (2009) reported 100% acceptance of bites offered at post intervention, but this reduced to 64% up to 2-years follow up. Outcomes from Paul et al. (2007) also reduced from 65 new foods eaten post-intervention to 53 at 3-month follow-up. However, some studies maintained benefits of the intervention at follow-up and in one study, the number of foods eaten continued to increase at follow-up compared to post intervention (Koegel et al. 2012).

Setting

Of the 19 studies carried out in clinical or laboratory settings, all reported positive effects. These interventions tended to include DRA, EE and NCR procedures and were generally longer in session duration, whereas the nine studies carried out in home settings (although still by a clinician) tended to include prompts, fading techniques and positive reinforcement. After clinician implemented interventions, a few studies used parental implementation of the same interventions which appears to be effective. Ewry and Fryling (2016) report that percentage of bites accepted (of non-preferred foods) maintained at 96% during parental implementation. Similarly, studies where only parents and not clinicians implemented interventions (after training) were also reported to be effective (Penrod et al. 2010; Seiverling et al. 2012b).

The remaining five studies took place in school settings. Two of these studies implemented environmental changes, such as portion-size modified plates, (Hubbard et al. 2015; Wallen et al. 2013) that were not effective. The other three studies implemented access to preferred foods and positive reinforcement (Levin and Carr 2001), demand fading and positive reinforcement (Barahona et al. 2013) and positive reinforcement using a game (Cassey et al. 2016), which were all reported as effective.

Discussion

This review was conducted to identify types of interventions used for increasing dietary variety and acceptance of fruits/vegetables in DD populations and to determine their effectiveness. It was found that a range of techniques have been used to increase dietary variety, which can be categorised into three groups based on operant conditioning, systematic desensitisation and environmental/family based interventions. Techniques from all of these groups have been reported to be effective (although environmental interventions were only effective when combined with family interventions) for increasing healthy eating on an individual, case-by-case basis, by increasing the number of new foods eaten, the percentage of bites accepted during a meal and the amount (weight) of new foods that have been consumed. It was found that many studies attempting to increase dietary variety applied a package of different interventions spanning the three categories, to encourage change in acceptance of food.

Generally, interventions were reported to be most effective when multiple components were used, there was an incentive through reinforcement such as access to toys, or when systematic desensitisation or escape extinction procedures were used. Multi-component and phased interventions also suggest that for each individual, there is a hierarchy of techniques that might be effective to increase acceptance and dietary variety. Although EE techniques have been consistently reported as most effective, exposure and reinforcement techniques should be tried before EE and physical guidance strategies due to ethical reasons and to avoid the possibility of adverse side effects of EE (Goh and Iwata 1994). This suggestion fits well within a clinical model, however it can be questioned whether multiple component interventions are more commonly implemented because individual cases are complex, or whether it is because some interventions simply do not work (for the individual or the population).

Furthermore, although effective, nearly half of the studies did not follow-up participants after the intervention, meaning that the long-term effectiveness of these interventions cannot be determined. Although in some instances follow-ups may not be possible, this is an important detail that is missing from the literature because if there is no continued benefit after the intervention stops, then it could be questioned whether these interventions would warrant being implemented. This is especially important due to the length of some studies without a reported follow-up (e.g. 113 trials; Hodges et al. 2017) as this may be deemed excessively long without examining or reporting the lasting effectiveness of the intervention. Possible confounding variables (e.g. a maturation effect) are also often not considered as a possibility for decreasing selective eating or food refusal (Bandini et al. 2017) in such long-term interventions.

Perhaps the most practically useful outcome from this review is that some environmental interventions, such as using special plates and changing the placement of foods in lunchrooms, are simply not effective in increasing diet variety in the DD population (Hubbard et al. 2015; Wallen et al. 2013). Although, the quality of these studies was poor (due to selection and detection biases) and it may be that better designed studies could have positive effects. Study design in general was highlighted as a major issue in this review for determining effectiveness of interventions. The review included only one randomized clinical trial, suggesting that number of foods eaten can be increased through ten-sessions of either operant conditioning or systematic desensitisation. However, 30 of the remaining studies were case studies or case series, which meant that the interventions delivered were highly personalised and could have included some of the more selective participants from the DD population, as they warranted clinical intervention. Consequently, the findings of these studies may not be generalizable to selective eaters drawn from the DD population unless they warrant clinical intervention. For these people, there is very little research specifically focused on eating a varied diet. It is only when the selective eating becomes a problem to physical health that interventions are delivered. This contrasts with healthy eating interventions in typically-developing people as these are generally aimed at improving public health and not predicated on clinical need (Mikkelsen et al. 2014). The current review helps to illustrate this difference as 33/34 positive studies were grounded in learning theory, involving strategies based on the use of applied behavioural analysis (ABA; Virués-Ortega 2010), a technique frequently used with people with ASD to shape preferred behaviours. Therefore findings of the current review are restricted to commenting on the individual with selective eating, rather than the population of selective eaters.

Limitations

Overall, the evidence was not sufficiently robust to determine the effectiveness of these strategies on a population level. Interventions were personalised and used multiple-components in different combinations, meaning that their effectiveness could not be compared adequately.

A central limitation of this review is the Risk of Bias (RoB) within and between studies. The studies were mostly rated as low RoB using the SCD-RoB Tool. However, many studies did not report the intensity or duration of intervention, intervention setting and many did not have a follow-up time point. All of which would suggest a high RoB that was not detected by the tool used. This also indicates that there is a high risk of selective outcome reporting. Similarly, there is a risk of publication bias due to the studies mostly utilising single-case designs and reporting high rates of effective interventions. This is particularly problematic because we do not have data for how many other participants interventions were tried within clinics but not effective and therefore not published.

Lastly, many studies included reducing problem mealtime behaviours as a secondary outcome. It was beyond the scope of the current review to examine these outcomes, although it would have been useful to use this information as a measure of disgust to the novel food stimuli. This is because it is difficult to determine from study reports how selective each person was before the intervention or what characteristics individuals may have that made them more or less resistant to change. Additionally, it is impossible to tell whether the outcome measure (number of foods consumed or percentage of bites eaten) was chosen based on how resistant to change or selective the participant was, or whether the measure was chosen based on feasibility, convenience or parental concern. Dependent on the reason, it could be questioned why the effectiveness of the same interventions has been measured using both macro and micro measures. This is because if the participant is more selective, it could be easier to show intervention effectiveness based on percentage of bites accepted rather than whole foods eaten.

Conclusion

Multiple component interventions based on operant conditioning, systematic desensitisation and combined environmental and family based interventions appear effective when applied to the individual. However, the effectiveness of these interventions for the population cannot be determined due to the majority of studies retrieved using a single-case design and employing different combinations of interventions in each study. Of the interventions identified, environmental changes at mealtimes appear to be less effective and less researched in this population, whereas behavioural interventions grounded by learning theory, such as systematic desensitisation, reinforcement and EE appear to be effective at increasing intake for a variety of novel foods and are therefore in greater evidence among published papers. However, it should be noted that there is a high risk of selective outcome reporting, publication bias and bias within and between studies included in this review.

Future Recommendations

There is a lack of interventions for DD populations to improve specifically variety of foods accepted and in particular FV intake. Systematic interventions with aims, duration and outcome measures laid out before delivering the interventions (pre-registration) are needed to determine whether individual interventions are effective. The interventions identified are also mostly used with DD populations, therefore it would be useful to determine whether interventions in which have been successful in typically-developing populations are also effective for people with DD, without clinically significant nutritional deficiencies. Lastly, it is important to establish whether these interventions are effective when delivered to a group or on a one-to-one basis, as many successful interventions to increase acceptance of novel foods in typically-developing populations are implemented in schools and not by clinicians.