Skip to main content
Log in

Predictors of Pragmatic Communication in School-Age Siblings of Children with ASD and Low-Risk Controls

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Little empirical evidence exists about school-age pragmatic communication or predictors in siblings at heightened familial risk for ASD (HR) and low-risk (LR) controls. The Pragmatic Rating Scale-School-Age (Landa unpublished) was scored for 49 HR siblings and 18 LR controls at 8–12 years. Social-communication and language measures were collected between 14 and 36 months. At 36-months, siblings were classified as ASD (HR-ASD, n = 15), broad autism phenotype (HR-BAP, n = 19), or typically developing (HR-TD, n = 15). Results revealed a pragmatic continuum with significantly better scores for HR-TD than HR-BAP or HR-ASD, and HR-BAP than HR-ASD. Per regression models including all participants, 14-month joint attention initiations predicted school-age pragmatic communication, as did 24-month social-communication and expressive language scores. Early joint attention, social-communication, and language abilities contribute to later pragmatic functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Yizhak, N., Yirmiya, N., Seidman, I., Alon, R., Lord, C., & Sigman, M. (2011). Pragmatic language and school related linguistic abilities in siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(6), 750–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC-2). London: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Mayberry, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., & Hallmayer, J. (2006). Characteristics of the broader phenotype in autism: A study of siblings using the Children’s Communication Checklist-2. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 141B, 117–122. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Young, G. S., Brian, J., Carter, A., Carver, L. J., Chawarska, K., et al. (2017). Non-ASD outcomes at 36 months in siblings at familial risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Autism Research, 10(1), 169–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.6.4.284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. (2011). The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatric Research, 69(5 PT 2), 55R–62R. https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, A. L., & Olswang, L. B. (2009). Investigating requests for information in children with autism spectrum disorders: Static versus dynamic assessment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 9(4), 297–311. https://doi.org/10.1080/14417040701413720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drumm, E., & Brian, J. (2013). The developing language abilities and increased risks of ‘unaffected’ siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiatry, 3(5), 513–524. https://doi.org/10.2217/NPY.13.65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drumm, E., Bryson, S., Zwaigenbaum, L., & Brian, J. (2015). Language-related abilities in ‘unaffected’ school-aged siblings of children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 18, 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(4), 297–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1977.tb00443.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Isaacson, T., & Summers, C. (2001). Social behaviors of children with language impairment on the playground: A pilot study. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 101–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Roberts, W., et al. (2013). A prospective study of autistic-like traits in unaffected siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(1), 42–48. https://doi.org/10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gessaroli, E., Santelli, E., di Pellegrino, G., & Frassinetti, F. (2013). Personal space regulation in childhood autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74959. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074959.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Khalulyan, A., del Rosario, M., McCarthy, B., Gomez, L., Sigman, M., et al. (2015). Is early joint attention associated with school-age pragmatic language? Autism, 19(2), 168–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism, 9(2), 157–178. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361305051395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thought and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, C. R., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Predictive and concurrent validity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 575–584.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven: Department of Sociology, Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, R. S., Losh, M., Parlier, M., Reznick, J. S., & Piven, J. (2007). The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1679–1690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy Krieger Institute. (2013). Early signs of autism video tutorial—Kennedy Krieger Institute [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YtvP5A5OHpU.

  • Klusek, J., Losh, M., & Martin, G. E. (2014). Sex differences and within-family associations in the broad autism phenotype. Autism, 18(2), 106–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klusek, J., Martin, G. E., & Losh, M. (2014). A comparison of pragmatic language in boys with autism and fragile X syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(5), 1692–1707.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, Y. G., & Yeung, S. S. S. (2012). Towards a convergent account of pragmatic language deficits in high-functioning autism: Depicting the phenotype using the Pragmatic Rating Scale. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 792–797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., Folstein, S. E., & Isaacs, C. (1991). Spontaneous narrative-discourse performance of parents of autistic individuals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 34(6), 1339–1345. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3406.1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O., Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychological Medicine, 22(01), 245–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2007). Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(7), 853–864. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., O’Neill, A. H., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(1), 13–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavery, M. R., Acharya, P., Sivo, S. A., & Xu, L. (2017). Number of predictors and multicollinearity: What are their effects on error and bias in regression. Communications in Statistics—Simulation and Computation. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610918.2017.1371750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (2nd ed.). Torrance: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., & Piven, J. (2007). Social-cognition and the broad autism phenotype: Identifying genetically meaningful phenotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(1), 105–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malesa, E., Foss-Feig, J., Yoder, P., Warren, Z., Walden, T., & Stone, W. (2012). Predicting language and social outcomes at age 5 for later-born siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 17(5), 558–570.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Young, G. S., Hutman, T., Johnson, S., Schwichtenberg, A. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2015). Early pragmatic language difficulties in siblings of children with autism: Implications for DSM-5 social communication disorder? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(7), 774–781. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A. (2009). A parallel and distributed processing model of joint attention, social-cognition and autism. Autism Research, 2(1), 2–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D. (2007). The Language Use Inventory for young children: A parent-report measure of pragmatic language development for 18- to 47-month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 50(1), 214–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52(3), 411–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G., Belding, A., Hill, M., Hill, A., Hutman, T., et al. (2014). The broader autism phenotype in infancy: When does it emerge? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(4), 398–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.020.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0607-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piven, J., Palmer, P., Landa, R., Santangelo, S., Jacobi, D., & Childress, D. (1997). Personality and language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 74(4), 398–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prutting, C. A., & Kirchner, D. M. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 105–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1984a). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: Part 1. Organizational framework and assessment parameters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(1), 2–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1984b). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: Part 2. Guidelines, considerations, and specific evaluation procedures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(1), 12–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, E. S., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. (2014). Assessing pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: The Yale in vivo Pragmatic Protocol. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2162–2173. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-14-0040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • statistics.laerd.com (2018). Principal components analysis (PCA): Background & requirements. Retrieved from https://statistics.laerd.com/premium/spss/pca/pca-in-spss-3.php.

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Caronna, E. (2007). Language disorders: Autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54, 469–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 132–158). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkstra, L. S., Clark, A., Burgess, S., Hengst, J. A., Wertheimer, J. C., & Paul, D. (2017). Pragmatic communication abilities in children and adults: Implications for rehabilitation professionals. Disability and Rehabilitation, 39(18), 1872–1885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E., Parlade, M. V., et al. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Virkud, Y. V., Todd, R. D., Abbacchi, A. M., Zhang, Y., & Constantino, J. N. (2009). Familial aggregation of quantitative autistic traits in multiplex versus simplex autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 150B(3), 328–334. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (2011). World report on disability. Geneva: WHO Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by Grant MH59630 from the National Institute of Mental Health (R.J.L., principal investigator). We also thank the many research clinicians who collected this study’s data, and the superb staff who provided administrative support, videotaped assessment sessions, and processed data. We also thank Luther Kalb for his expert consultation in nonparametric statistical analysis. Most importantly, we thank the participants and their families, who made this study possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RJL conceived of the study and obtained the funding. KJG and RJL planned the analyses. KJG and EAU coded the PRS-SA data, with input from RJL. KJG conducted the analyses. KJG and RJL interpreted the results. KJG took the lead in writing the manuscript. RJL provided critical feedback and helped shape the manuscript. EAU read and agreed with the contents of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca J. Landa.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

No conflicts of interest are declared.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All families gave written informed consent for child participation; at school-age, children provided verbal assent.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Greenslade, K.J., Utter, E.A. & Landa, R.J. Predictors of Pragmatic Communication in School-Age Siblings of Children with ASD and Low-Risk Controls. J Autism Dev Disord 49, 1352–1365 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3837-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3837-x

Keywords

Navigation