Skip to main content
Log in

Parents’ Translations of Child Gesture Facilitate Word Learning in Children with Autism, Down Syndrome and Typical Development

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Author Correction to this article was published on 14 October 2017

This article has been updated

Abstract

Typically-developing (TD) children frequently refer to objects uniquely in gesture. Parents translate these gestures into words, facilitating children’s acquisition of these words (Goldin-Meadow et al. in Dev Sci 10(6):778–785, 2007). We ask whether this pattern holds for children with autism (AU) and with Down syndrome (DS) who show delayed vocabulary development. We observed 23 children with AU, 23 with DS, and 23 TD children with their parents over a year. Children used gestures to indicate objects before labeling them and parents translated their gestures into words. Importantly, children benefited from this input, acquiring more words for the translated gestures than the not translated ones. Results highlight the role contingent parental input to child gesture plays in language development of children with developmental disorders.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 14 October 2017

    The original version of the article unfortunately contained mistake in the “Methods/Participants” section (p. 224) as “[M TD = 168.26 (SD = 125.18) vs. M AU = 172.91 (SD = 195.88) vs. M DS = 145.43 (SD = 88.78); Kruskal–Wallis, χ 2(2) = 2.42, p = .30], and for word types [M TD = 28.43 (SD = 26.89) vs. M AU = 39.65 (SD = 49.07) vs. M DS = 18.35 (SD = 22.78); χ 2(2) = 3.15, p = .21]’’. The correct text is given below: “[M TD = 51.91 (SD = 59.68) vs. M AU = 74.43 (SD = 116.01) vs. M DS = 25.26 (SD = 39.39); Kruskal–Wallis, χ 2(2) = 3.39, p = .18], and for word types [M TD = 18.48 (SD = 20.51) vs. M AU = 24.74 (SD = 32.98) vs. M DS  = 11.22 (SD = 18.87); χ 2(2) = 3.58, p = .17]”..

References

  • Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). The development of displaced speech in early mother-child conversations. Child Development, 77, 186–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., & Deckner, D. F. (2004). The development of symbol-infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75, 1171–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F., & Nelson, P. B. (2012). Rating parent–child interactions: Joint engagement, communication dynamics, and shared topics in autism, Down syndrome, and typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2622–2635.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F., & Romski, M. A. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 84–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aldred, C., Green, J., & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication intervention for children with autism: Pilot randomized controlled treatment study suggesting effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1420–1430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition (text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1987). An organizational approach to symbolic development in children with Down syndrome. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Atypical Symbolic Development (pp. 5–30). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrini, B., & Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. European Psychiatry, 18, 6–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism, 2(4), 325–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1125–1135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of child language (pp. 641–663). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colgan, S., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L., Crais, E., & Baranek, G. (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 307–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I. M. (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Research, 3, 311–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). The MacArthur communicative development inventories: Users’ guide and technical manual. San Diego: Singular Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F., & Wishart, J. (1995). Use of pointing and other gestures by young children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100, 160–182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Learning through gesture. WIREs (Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews). Cognitive Science, 2(6), 595–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Widening the lens: What the manual modality reveals about language, learning, and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 369(1651), 20130295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10(6), 778–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). “I beg your pardon?” The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13, 455–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., et al. (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomized controlled trial. Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gulsrud, A. C., Hellemann, G. S., Freeman, S. F. N., & Kasari, C. (2014). Two to ten years: Developmental trajectories of joint attention in children with ASD who received targeted social communication interventions. Autism Research, 7, 207–2015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haebig, E., McDuffie, A., & Weismer, S. E. (2013). Brief report: Parent verbal responsiveness and language development in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(9), 2218–2227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (2003). Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 179–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leezenbaum, N. B., Campbell, C. B., Butler, D., & Iverson, J. M. (2014). Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism, 18(6), 694–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1426–1438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcos, H. (1991). How adults contribute to the development of early referential communication? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 6(3), 271–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Merill-Palmer Quarterly, 8, 23–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDuffie, A., & Yoder, P. (2010). Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1026–1039.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. F. (1999). Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome. In J. Miller, M. Leddy, & L. Leavitt (Eds.), Improving the communication of people with Down syndrome (pp. 11–40). Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Smith, I., et al. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, S69–S78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1995). Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 157–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, 115–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2011). Infants’ gestures influence mothers’ provision of object, action and internal state labels. Journal of Child Language, 38, 1028–1054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., Adamson, L. B., & Dimitrova, N. (in press). Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism: International Journal of Research and Practice.

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., Bailey, J., & Schmuck, L. (2015). Baby sign but not spontaneous gesture predicts later vocabulary in children with Down Syndrome. Journal of Child Language. doi:10.1017/S030500091500029X.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalıskan, Ş., & Dimitrova, N. (2013). How gesture input provides a helping hand to language development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 155–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 481–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96(3), B101–B113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). Role of gesture in children’s early constructions. In E. Clark & B. Kelly (Eds.), Constructions in acquisition (pp. 31–58). Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Transactional regulation: The developmental ecology of early intervention. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. 135–159). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., Hutman, T., & Sigman, M. (2013). A parent-mediated intervention to increase responsive parental behaviors and child communication in children with ASD: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 540–555.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 77–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1691–1704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer Harris, N., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down Syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 345–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V., Mirenda, P., & Zaidman-Zait, A. (2007). Predictors of expressive vocabulary growth in children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 149–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanini, S., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (2007). Spoken and gestural production in a naming task by young children with Down syndrome. Brain and Language, 101(3), 208–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in 2- and 3-year old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 677–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venker, C., McDuffie, A., Weismer, S., & Abbeduto, L. (2011). Increasing verbal responsiveness in parents of children with autism: A pilot study. Autism, 16(6), 568–585.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2000). Autism spectrum disorders: A transactional developmental perspective. Baltimore: Paul Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., Prizant, B., & Hutchinson, T. (1998). Communicative, social-affective, and symbolic profiles of young children with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Americal Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 79–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, M. J. (1992). Enhancing initial communication skills in young children with developmental disabilities through partner programming. Seminars in Speech and Hearing, 13, 194–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, M. J., Shannon, M. S., & Bacon, C. K. (1992). Longer term outcomes of prelinguistic intervention. Paper presented at the Division for Early Childhood Meeting, Washington, DC.

  • Yoder, P., Watson, L. R., & Lambert, W. (2015). Value-added predictors of expressive and receptive language growth in initially nonverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(5), 1254–1270.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini, L. (2008). Produzione gestuale e pre-verbale a 24 mesi in bambini con sindrome di Down e bambini parlatori tardivi [Gestural and pre-verbal communication in 24-month-old children with Down syndrome and late-talking children]. Psicologia clinica dello sviluppo, 3, 511–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zampini, L., & D’Odorico, L. (2011). Gesture production and language development. A longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome. Gesture, 11(2), 174–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool language scale (4th ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by grants by Swiss National Science Foundation (PBLAP1_142782, PI: Dimitrova), National Science Foundation (BCS 1251337, PI: Özçalışkan), and National Institutes of Health (R01 HD035612, PI: Adamson). We thank the participating families for their dedication to our research efforts. We also thank Lauren Schmuck and Jhonelle Bailey for their help in coding the data, along with the Action Editor and the reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

All authors participated in the conception and design of the study, in the interpretation of the findings, and in the drafting of the manuscript. In addition, ND compiled the data and performed the statistical analysis, SO supervised the coding of child gestures, and LBA supervised the use of the archive of video records and verbal transcripts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nevena Dimitrova.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

This study was approved by an ethics committee and has therefore been performed in accordance with the ethical standards specified in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. All parents provided informed consent for their and their child’s participation prior to their inclusion in the study.

Additional information

A correction to this article is available online at https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3346-3.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş. & Adamson, L.B. Parents’ Translations of Child Gesture Facilitate Word Learning in Children with Autism, Down Syndrome and Typical Development. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 221–231 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2566-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2566-7

Keywords

Navigation