Skip to main content
Log in

Anticipation of Action Intentions in Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated whether individuals with a mild form of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are influenced by an actor’s gaze direction when anticipating how an observed action will continue in the immediate future. Participants observed a head rotate towards them, while the gaze direction was either leading, or lagging behind, rotation. They also observed identical rotations of a cylinder containing the geometrical equivalent of the gaze manipulation. The control group was influenced by the gaze manipulations for the animate but not the inanimate stimulus. The ASD group did not discriminate between the stimuli, showing a similar influence for both. This suggests that the ASD responses in the animate condition were biased by the low-level directional features of the eyes rather than by the conveyed intentions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Pierno, A., Mari, M., Lusher, D., & Castiello, U. (2007). Motor contagion from gaze: The case of autism. Brain, 130, 2401–2411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castiello, U. (2003). Understanding other people’s actions: Intention and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 29, 416–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. Child Development, 74, 1108–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Entremont, B., & Yazbek, A. (2007). Imitation of intentional and accidental actions by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1665–1678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, J. G., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Butcher, B., & Walkowiak, J. (2008). Brief report: Attention effect on a measure of social perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1797–1802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424, 769–771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 10, 126–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frischen, A., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Long-term gaze cueing effects: Evidence for retrieval of prior states of attention from memory. Visual Cognition, 14, 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36, T22–T32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioural and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 290–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands, M. A., Patla, A. E., & Vickers, J. N. (2002). Look where you’re going!: Gaze behaviour associated with maintaining and changing the direction of locomotion. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 221–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, M., & Jellema, T. (2011). Resolving ambiguous behavioral intentions by means of involuntary prioritization of gaze processing. Emotion, 11, 681–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, M., Liu, C. H., & Jellema, T. (2009). Anticipating intentional actions: The effect of gaze direction on the judgment of head rotation. Cognition, 112, 423–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarraya, M., Amorim, M. A., & Bardy, B. G. (2005). Optical flow and viewpoint change modulate the perception and memorization of complex motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 951–961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden actions. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action, Attention and Performance, 19, 356–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Neural pathways of social cognition. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., Lorteije, J., van Rijn, S., van ‘t Wout, M., de Haan, E., van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary interpretation of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farroni, T., De Haan, M., et al. (2005). The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 599–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemner, C., Schuller, A. M., & van Engeland, H. (2006). Electrocortical reflections of face and gaze processing in children with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1063–1072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Garver, C. R., Grannemann, B. D., Andrews, A. A., Savla, J. S., et al. (2006). The pattern of sensory processing abnormalities in autism. Autism, 10, 480–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kylliainen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 435–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Land, M., Mennie, N., & Rusted, J. (1999). The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception, 28, 1311–1328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., Baron-Cohen, S., Perrett, D., Milders, M., & Brown, S. D. (1997). Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51, 77–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leekham, S. R., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 951–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leekham, S. R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36, 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Penny, S. (2008). Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: Is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Development and Psychopathology, 20, 79–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2006a). When gaze turns into grasp. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2130–2137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierno, A. C., Mari, M., Glover, S., Georgiou, I., & Castiello, U. (2006b). Failure to read motor intentions from gaze in children with autism. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1483–1488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redcay, E. (2008). The superior temporal sulcus performs a common function for social and speech perception: Implications for the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 123–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., Mottron, L., Friesen, C. K., Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Eyes are special but not for everyone: The case of autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 715–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rotman, G., Troje, N. F., Johansson, R. S., & Flanagan, J. R. (2006). Eye movements when observing predictable and unpredictable actions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 1358–1369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., & Rideout, P. (2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(8), 1083–1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 139–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009). The eye contact effect: Mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 127–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 445–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, J., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Autistic children understand seeing and wanting. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 163–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, I. M., & Hayes, A. E. (2004). Anticipating action in complex scenes. Visual Cognition, 11, 341–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van ‘t Wout, M., van Rijn, S., Jellema, T., Kahn, R. S., & Aleman, A. (2009). Deficits in implicit attention to social signals in schizophrenia and high risk groups: Behavioural evidence from a new illusion. PloS One, 4(E5581), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Stauder, J. E. A., van Son, I. A. M., & Mottron, L. (2005). Atypical visual orienting to gaze- and arrow-cues in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 267–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale-third edition: administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al. (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, R. M., Wann, J. P., & Allison, R. S. (2008). Active gaze, visual look-ahead, and locomotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1150–1164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., Lancaster, J., & Emmorey, K. (2010). Representational momentum for the human body: Awkwardness matters, experience does not. Cognition, 116, 242–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of diseases clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zalla, T., Sav, A. M., Stopin, A., Ahade, S., & Leboyer, M. (2009). Faux pas detection and intentional action in Asperger syndrome: A replication in a French sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 373–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zalla, T., Labruyere, N., Clement, A., & Georgieff, N. (2010). Predicting ensuing actions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 809–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilbovicius, M., Meresse, I., Chabane, N., Brunelle, F., Samson, Y., & Boddaert, N. (2006). Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 359–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the AHRC and the European Science Foundation (Eurocores).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Hudson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hudson, M., Burnett, H.G. & Jellema, T. Anticipation of Action Intentions in Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 1684–1693 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1410-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1410-y

Keywords

Navigation