Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring the Nature of Joint Attention Impairments in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Associated Social and Cognitive Skills

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is generally accepted that joint attention skills are impaired in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In this study, social preference, attention disengagement and intention understanding, assumed to be associated with the development of joint attention, are explored in relation to joint attention skills in children with ASD at the age of 36 months. Response to joint attention was related to intention understanding, whereas the number of joint attention initiations was associated with attention disengagement, and somewhat less stronger with social preference. The level on which children initiated joint attention was related to social preference. Possible interpretations of these findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3(3), 294–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 328–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bono, M. A., Daley, T., & Sigman, M. (2004). Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention, and language gain in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 495–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. In D. Frye & C. Moore (Eds.), Children’s theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 49–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589–599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cassia, V. M., Valenza, E., Simion, F., & Leo, I. (2008). Congruency as a nonspecific perceptual property contributing to newborns’ face preference. Child Development, 79(4), 807–820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G. (2002). Human beings, animals and inanimate objects. What do people with autism like? Autism, 6(1), 93–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, S. M., & Dissayanake, C. (2008). The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 791–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Entremont, B., & Yazbek, A. (2007). Imitation of intentional and accidental actions by children with autism. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1665–1678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Bildt, A., Systema, S., van Lang, N. D. J., Minderaa, R. B., van Engeland, H., & de Jonge, M. V. (2009). Evaluation of the ADOS Revised Algorithm: The applicability in 558 Dutch children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(9), 1350–1358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deák, G. O., Flom, R. A., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 511–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delinicolas, E. K., & Young, R. L. (2007). Joint attention, language, social relating, and stereotypical behaviours in children with autistic disorder. Autism, 11(5), 425–436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Holmboe, K., Tucker, L., Csibra, G., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2009). Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: Disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(5), 637–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(47), 17245–17250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., Leekam, S. R., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiologia, 47(1), 248–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37, 571–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). Review article: The development of the social brain in human infancy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 909–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal-ability in the theory of mind task-performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. A., Carr, E. G., & Feeley, K. M. (2006). Multiple effects of joint attention intervention for children with autism. Behavior Modification, 30(6), 782–834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disablility in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 611–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Paparella, T., Freeman, S., & Jahromi, L. B. (2008). Language outcome in autism: Random comparison of joint attention and play interventions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(1), 125–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society, Series B, 358, 345–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Carter, C. M. (1998). Pivotal responses and the natural language teaching paradigm. Seminars in Speech and Language, 19, 355–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwisthout, J., Vogt, P., Haselager, P., & Dijkstra, T. (2008). Joint attention and language evolution. Connection Science, 20, 155–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R. (2005). Why do children with autism have a joint attention impairment. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., Hunnisett, M., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(7), 951–962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., & Ramsden, C. A. H. (2006). Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 185–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M., Markova, G., Marsh, H., & Barac, R. (2006). Distinguishing between intentions and outcomes in 5–7-month-olds. Poster presented at XVth Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Kyoto, Japan.

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism diagnostic schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Cesari, A., Paziente, A., Stern, D., et al. (2005). How young children treat objects and people: An emperical study of the first year of life in autism. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 35(4), 383–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239–1245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. L., Stavroupoulos, J., Nienhuis, T., & Legerstee, M. (2010). Six- and 9-month-old infants discriminate between goals despite similar action patterns. Infancy, 15(1), 94–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod Turner, L. (2005). Social and nonsocial orienting in young children with autism, developmental disorders, and typical development, Dissertation.

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social-emotional approach behaviour in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 63–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (2003). Annotation: The neural basis of social impairments in autism: The role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 793–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Vaughan van Hecke, A., & Venezia Parlade, M. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Burnette, C. (2005). Joint attention and neurodevelopmental models of autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 650–681). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early social communication scales (ESCS). Available through the University of Miami Psychology Department, Coral Gables, Florida.

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (2006) Joint attention, social competence, and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti, D. Cohen (Eds.). Developmental psychopathology, 2nd edn, Vol. 1: Theory and methods. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

  • Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2009). A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism Research, 2, 2–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. S., Creaghead, N. A., Manning-Courtney, P., Shear, P. K., Bean, J., & Prendeville, J. (2008). The relationship between joint attention and language in children with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 5–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naber, F. B. A., Swinkels, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., Dietz, C., van Daalen, E., Bakermans-Kramerburg, M. J., et al. (2007). Joint attention and attachment in toddlers with autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 17, 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Penny, S. (2008). Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: Is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Development and Psychopathology, 20, 79–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noldus Information Technology. (2003). The Observer: Professional system for collection, analysis, presentation and management of observational data. Reference Manual, Version 7.0. Wageningen, The Netherlands: Author.

  • Oosterling, I., Roos, S., de Bildt, A., Rommelse, N., de Jonge, M., Visser, J., et al. (2010). Improved diagnostic validity of the ADOS revised algorithms: A replication study in an independent sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(6), 689–703. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0915-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2006). Exogenous and endogenous attention orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 361–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roos, E. M., McDuffie, A. S., Weismer, S. E., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). A comparison of contexts for assessing joint attention in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Autism, 12(3), 275–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N., Tsuchiya, N., Hurley, R., Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Adolphs, R., et al. (2007). Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2580–2588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaerlaekens, A., Zink, I., & Van Ommeslaeghe, K. (2003). Reynell Taalontwikkelingsschalen, Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger.

  • Schilbach, L., Wilms, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Romanzetti, S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., et al. (2009). Minds made for sharing. An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates of joint attention during engagement in online social interaction. NeuroImage, 47(Suppl 1), S39–S41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691–1704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., & McConnell, D. (2003). Emergence of joint attention: Relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(1), 54–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Report: Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science, 8(4), 333–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., Stahl, D., & Cleveland, A. (2009). Taking a closer look at social and cognitive skills: A weekly longitudinal assessment between 7 and 10 months of age. European Journal of Developmental Psychopathology, 6(5), 567–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind and Language, 18(2), 121–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 993–1005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., & Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 892–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan Van Hecke, A., Mundy, P., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., et al. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vismara, L. A., & Lyons, G. L. (2007). Using perseverative interests to elicit joint attention behaviors in young children with autism: Theoretical and clinical implications for understanding motivation. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9(4), 214–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warreyn, P., Roeyers, H., Oelbrandt, T., & De Groote, I. (2005). What are you looking at? Joint attention and visual perspective taking in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 17(1), 55–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warreyn, P., Roeyers, H., Van Wetswinkel, U., & De Groote, I. (2007). Temporal coordination of joint attention behavior in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 501–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., Watt, N., Morgan, L., & Shumway, S. (2007). Social communication profiles of children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 960–975.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analysis comparing ToM abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normal developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the parents and children for participating in the study and making this research possible. This study was supported by a grant from the Research Foundation Flanders, Belgium (FWO).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inge Schietecatte.

Appendix

Appendix

See Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Examples of social (a) and nonsocial stimuli (b) used in the social preference task

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schietecatte, I., Roeyers, H. & Warreyn, P. Exploring the Nature of Joint Attention Impairments in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Associated Social and Cognitive Skills. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 1–12 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1209-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1209-x

Keywords

Navigation