Skip to main content
Log in

Scalar Inferences in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

On being told “John or Mary will come”, one might infer that not both of them will come. Yet the semantics of “or” is compatible with a situation where both John and Mary come. Inferences of this type, which enrich the semantics of “or” from an ‘inclusive’ to an ‘exclusive’ interpretation, have been extensively studied in linguistic pragmatics. However, the phenomenon has not been much explored in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs), where pragmatic deficits are commonly reported. Here, we present an experiment investigating these inferences. We predicted that, as a result of the reported pragmatic deficits, participants with ASD would produce fewer inferential enrichments of “or” than matched controls. However, contrary to expectations, but in line with recent findings by Pijnacker et al. (Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618, 2009), performances did not differ across groups. This unexpected finding is discussed in light of the literature on pragmatic abilities in autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Alternative, more semantic, treatments are suggested in recent work by Chiercha and his colleagues (Chierchia, 2004, to appear).

  2. These studies have also shown that children are more likely to draw implicatures when task demands are reduced, which suggests that limitations in cognitive resources—rather than diminished pragmatic competence—may play an important role in children’s pragmatic difficulties (Siegal and Surian 2004).

  3. This rate of pragmatic enrichment (leading to an exclusive interpretation) is among the highest reported in laboratory tasks dealing with scalar terms.

  4. The correlation between VIQ and scalar inference rate is not reported for the ASD group as a whole. This correlation is not found in the AS group or in the TD group, both ps > .1.

  5. “A and B” is only true if both A and B are true, whereas “A or B” is true if A alone is true, or if B alone is true, or if both A and B are true when an inclusive reading of the disjunction is chosen.

References

  • APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asperger, H. (1944). Die „Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 117(1), 76–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C. A. M., & Guthrie, D. (1987). The use of primary sentence stress by normal, aphasic, and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), 255–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A 15-year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–21). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bott, L., & Noveck, I. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(3), 437–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M., & Rumain, B. (1981). Development of comprehension of `Or’: Evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31(1), 46–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, B., & Tagliapietra, L. (in press). The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes.

  • Breheny, R., Katsos, N., & Williams, J. (2006). Are generalised scalar implicatures generated by default? An on-line investigation into the role of context in generating pragmatic inferences. Cognition, 100(3), 434–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burton-Roberts, N. (2007). Pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave-Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carston, R. (1998). Informativeness, relevance and scalar implicature. Relevance Theory: Applications and Implications, 1, 79–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carston, R. (2002). Thoughts and utterances: The pragmatics of explicit communication. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F., & Wilson, D. (2009). From acoustics to grammar: Perceiving and interpreting grammatical prosody in adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 502–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Nazir, T., Bott, L., Lanzetti, V., & Sperber, D. (2008). Making disjunctions exclusive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(11), 1741–1760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (2004). Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond (pp. 39–103). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G., Fox, D., & Spector, B. (to appear). The grammatical view of scalar implicatures and the relationship between semantics and pragmatics The handbook of semantics.

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W., & Schaeken, W. (2007). When people are more logical under cognitive load: Dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 128–133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, J., Watson, D., Bennetto, L., McDonough, J., & Gunlogson, C. (2009). An acoustic analysis of prosody in high-functioning autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(03), 385–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Whetton, K. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: National Foundation for Education Research—Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Findlay, J. M. (1978). Estimates on probability functions: A more virulent PEST. Perception and Psychophysics, 23(2), 181–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J. (2005). The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(4), 409–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1998). What autism teaches us about communication. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 23(2), 51–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 346(1315), 97–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, B. (2009). Scalar implicature and local pragmatics. Mind & Language, 24, 51–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J., & Rutherford, M. (2007). The ‘reading the mind in the voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1096–1106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, S. B., & Salkind, N. J. (2008). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analysing and understanding data (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English. Los Angeles: University of California Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, L. (2004). Implicature. In L. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 4–28). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, L., & Ward, G. (2004). The handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • House, J. (2006). Constructing a context with intonation. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1542–1558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, D. C. (1997). Statistical methods for psychology (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huckvale, M. (2003). Prorec (version 1.0). University College London, Downloaded from http://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/resource/prorec/#download.

  • Huckvale, M. (2004). Speech Filing System suite (version 4.6). University College London, Downloaded from http://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/resource/sfs/.

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30(5), 1169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), 807–828.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Schultz, R., & Cohen, D. (2000). Theory of mind in action: Developmental perspectives on social neuroscience. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 357–388). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature: Mit Pr.

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Langauge, 34(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S., Jessica Brian, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Isabel Smith, P., et al. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78(2), 165–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I., & Posada, A. (2003). Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study. Brain and Language, 85(2), 203–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I., & Reboul, A. (2008). Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 425–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2004). Experimental pragmatics. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2007). The why and how of experimental pragmatics: The case of ‘scalar inferences’. In N. Burton-Roberts (Ed.), Pragmatics (pp. 184–212). Basingstoke: Palgrave.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86(3), 253–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G. (1973). Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16(2), 278–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J.-P., & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A. (2007). Processing costs and their impact on the development of scalar implicature. Language Acquisition, 14(4), 347–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recanati, F. (2003). Embedded implicatures. Philosophical Perspectives, 17, 299–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger Syndrome and High Functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1097–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2004). Conceptual development and conversational understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 534–538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N., Hermelin, B., & Tsimpli, I. (2003). Dissociation of social affect and theory of mind in a case of Asperger Syndrome. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 15, 303–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (accepted). 17-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ communication. Developmental Science.

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17(1), 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. (1979). Developmental patterns in the encoding and combination of logical connectives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28(3), 469–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean Maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2008). Language and communication in autism and Asperger Syndrome. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutcliffe, P., & Bishop, D. (2005). Psychophysical design influences frequency discrimination performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(3), 249–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1, 335–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2007). Reading affect in the face and voice: Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(6), 698.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49(3), 367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WHO. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. (2004a). Not just anything: A collection of thoughts on paper. London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. (2004b). Poetry and Prose Retrieved 8th January 2008, from http://www.donnawilliams.net/poetryprose.0.html.

  • Williams, K., Tuck, M., Helmer, M., Bartak, L., Mellis, C., & Peat, J. (2008). Diagnostic labelling of Autism Spectrum Disorders in NSW. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 44(3), 108–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. (in press). Pragmatic processes and metarepresentational abilities: The case of verbal irony. In T. Matsui (Ed.), Pragmatics and theory of mind. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

  • Wilson, D., & Wharton, T. (2006). Relevance and prosody. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1559–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witwer, A., & Lecavalier, L. (2008). Examining the validity of autism spectrum disorder subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 1611–1624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the children and staff in North Hill House (Frome, UK), Southlands (Lymington, UK), Henry Fanshaw School (Dronfield, UK), Chelmer Valley High School (Chelmsford, UK) and Haberdasher’s Aske’s Boys School (Herts, UK).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Coralie Chevallier.

Additional information

These studies were conducted through a collaboration between the Laboratoire Langage, Cerveau et Cognition, the Institute of Psychiatry and UCL’s Department of Phonetics and Linguistics.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

See (Table 4).

Table 4 Complete list of stimuli

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chevallier, C., Wilson, D., Happé, F. et al. Scalar Inferences in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40, 1104–1117 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0960-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0960-8

Keywords

Navigation