Skip to main content
Log in

Assessment of Stimulus Overselectivity with Tactile Compound Stimuli in Children with Autism

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Autistic and typical children mastered a simultaneous discrimination task with three sets of all-tactile compound stimuli. During training, responding to one stimulus (S+) resulted in rewards whereas responding to the alternative (S−) was extinguished. Test 1 was conducted with recombinations of S+ and S− elements. In Test 2, the test stimulus to which the child responded most in Test 1 was pitched against the training S+. In Test 1, all children responded exclusively to one test probe, spuriously implying stimulus overselectivity in both populations. However, in Test 2, the typical children responded mostly to the training S+ indicating control by both S+ elements; the autistic children responded to both stimuli indicating reduced control by the second S+ element (indicating overselectivity).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ameli, R., Courchesne, E., & Lincoln, A. (1988). Visual memory processes in high-functioning individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 601–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1980) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revised). Washington: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, S. L. (1981). Stimulus overselectivity in learning disabled children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 239–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (Vol. 23). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauman, M. L. (1996). Brief report: Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 199–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J. M. (2002). The existential theory of mind. Review of General Psychology, 6, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Richmond, G., Bell, J., & Brown, K. (1986). A microanalysis of the controlling stimulus–response relations engendered during the assessment of stimulus overselectivity. The Psychological Record, 36, 225–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Stella, M. E., & Etzel, B. C. (1984). A reevaluation of stimulus overselectivity: Restricted stimulus control or stimulus control hierarchies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 137–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brack, U. B. (2001). Überselektive Wahrnehmung bei retardierten Kindern. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceponiene, R., Lepisto, T., Shestakova, A., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Näätänen, R., et al. (2003). Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: They can perceive but do not attend. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 5567–5572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 315–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski, K. T., Courchesne, E., & Elmasian, R. (1990). Effects of focused selective attention tasks on event-related potential in autistic and normal individuals. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, I. L. (1998). Neural network analysis of learning in autism. In D. J. Stein, & J. Ludik (Eds.), Neural networks and psychopathology (pp. 273–314). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Hesselink, J. R., Jernigan, T. L., & Yeung-Courchesne, R. (1987). Abnormal neuroanatomy in a nonretarded person with autism: Unusual findings with magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of Neurology, 44, 335–341.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A. J., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Elmasian, R., et al. (1989). Pathophysiologic findings in nonretarded autism and receptive developmental language disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 848–865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Hicks, G., & Lincoln, A. J. (1985). Functioning of the brain-stem auditory pathway in non-retarded autistic individuals. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 61, 491–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbot, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dirlich-Wilhelm, H., & Müller, M. (1982). Extreme Reizselektion—eine Funktion des Entwicklungsalters. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, Forschung, und Praxis, 11, 177–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P. E. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11, 467–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlap, G., Koegel, R. L., & Burke, J. C. (1981). Educational implications of stimulus overselectivity in autistic children. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 2, 37–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairbank, D., Powers, A., & Monaghan, C. (1986). Stimulus overselectivity in hearing-impaired children. Volta Review, 88, 269–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grillon, C., Courchesne, E., & Akshoomoff, N. A. (1989). Brainstem and middle latency auditory evoked potentials in autism and developmental language disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 255–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 543–551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., & Capps, L. M. (2003). Making sense of self-conscious emotion: Linking theory of mind and emotion in children with autism. Emotion, 3, 394–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. (1975). Maximizing and matching on concurrent ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 107–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrod, C., Butler, D. W., Cottington, E. M., & Jiminez, F. (2000). Linking theory of mind and central coherence bias in autism and in the general population. Developmental Psychology, 36, 126–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L., & Eisenberg, J. (1956). Early infantile autism 1943–1955. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 26, 55–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. (1998). Neuropathology of infantile autism. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 57, 645–651.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., Good, A. B., Cerniglia, L., Murphy, C., & Koegel, L. K. (1989). How to teach pivotal behaviors to autistic children: A training manual. Santa Barbara: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kremer-Sadlik, T. (2004). How children with autism and Asperger Syndrome respond to questions: A ‘naturalistic’ theory of mind task. Discourse Studies, 6, 185–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., & López, B. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36, 261–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson’s choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R. L., & Rehm, R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 211–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B., Shute, R., & Rees, R. (2001). An analysis of stimulus overselectivity in adults with autism. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 26, 161–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 325–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology, 52, 917–922.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, B., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak central coherence, poor joint attention, and low verbal ability: Independent deficits in early autism. Developmental Psychology, 39, 646–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murakami, J. W., Courchesne, E., & Press, G. A. (1989). Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in autism. Archives of Neurology, 46, 689–694.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 205–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J. M., Aydin, A., & Redhead, E. S. (1997). Configural analysis of summation in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 84–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piven, J., Saliba, K., Bailey, J., & Arndt, S. (1997). An MRI study of autism: The cerebellum revisited. Neurology, 49, 546–551.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 203–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. Rutter, & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatments (pp. 1–25). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, H. C., & Salzberg, C. L. (1982). Stimulus overselectivity in a match-to-sample paradigm by severely retarded youth. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 273–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreibman, L., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Britten, K. R. (1986). Differential responding to content and intonation components of a complex auditory stimulus by nonverbal and echolalic autistic children. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 109–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Blades, M. (2003). Language and auditory processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 378–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 456–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, H., & Lovaas, O. I. (1976). Stimulus overselectivity: A common feature in autism and mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 81, 26–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Solomonica-Levi, D., & Shulman, C. (1996). The ability to manipulate behavior and to understand manipulation of beliefs: A comparison of individuals with autism, mental retardation, and normal development. Developmental Psychology, 32, 62–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowldgements

The authors wish to thank the staff of Eden II and the children and their families for their support and participation in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bertram O. Ploog.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ploog, B.O., Kim, N. Assessment of Stimulus Overselectivity with Tactile Compound Stimuli in Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 1514–1524 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0244-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0244-5

Keywords

Navigation