Skip to main content
Log in

A Longitudinal Study of Pretend Play in Autism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study describes a longitudinal design (following subjects described in Rutherford & Rogers [2003, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 33, 289–302]) to test for predictors of pretend play competence in a group of children with autism. We tested the hypothesis that developmental change in pretend play performance can be predicted by earlier measures of either executive function, intersubjectivity, imitation, or general development. Participants at the time of follow-up testing were 28 children with autistic disorder (mean chronological age (CA) 57.6 months), 18 children with other developmental disorders (mean CA 59.0 months), and 27 typically developing children (mean CA 30.1 months). Children with autism were profoundly delayed given both competence (prompted) measures as well as performance (spontaneous) measures. Joint attention at time 1 strongly and uniquely predicted pretend play development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The same analyses done using play scores at time 1 as a true covariate (i.e., entered into the model even before group diagnosis) yielded the same results, albeit with the expected drop in power when using additional variables).

  2. It should be noted however, that for the AD group, when analyzed separately, there was a significant relationship between JA and Imitation (r = .44, P = .03).

  3. Note that in a hierarchical regression in which MA, imitation and spatial reversal are forced in before JA, JA still predicts a significant amount of variance in spontaneous pretend play development (P = .002) whereas no other predictor, while forced in ahead of JA, predicts a significant amount of variance.

References

  • Astington, J., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernabei, P., Camaioni, L., & Levi, G. (1999). An evaluation of early development in children with autism and pervasive developmental disorders from home movies: Preliminary findings. Autism, 2, 243–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, D. P., Baker, L., Rutter, M., & Mawhood, L. (1989). Infantile autism and developmental receptive dysphasia: A comparative follow-up into middle childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 19–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, C., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 265–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. Y., Luk, S. L., & Lee, P. W. (1990). A follow-up study of infantile autism in Hong Kong. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 221–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 281–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., & Estes, A. (2004). Defining the early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and responses to emotions. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Norton, J. A., Allen, J., & Steel, R. (1973). Prognosis in autism: A follow-up study. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 42–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, M. B., & Rosenfeld, A. A. (1984). Play assessment in the differential diagnosis of autism and other causes of severe language disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 5, 26–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, L. (1956). The autistic child in adolescence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 607–612.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Development, 52, 1095–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fewell, R. R., & Rich, J. S. (1987). Play assessment as a procedure for examining cognitive, communication, and social skills in multihandicapped children. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 5, 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fidell, L. S., & Tabachnick, B. G. (2003). Preparatory data analysis. In J. A. Schinka, & W. F. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Research methods in psychology (pp. 115–141). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C. (1991). Play. (2nd ed.) London: Fontana Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, S. (1987). Outcome and prognostic factors in infantile autism and similar conditions: A population-based study of 46 cases followed through puberty. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 273–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J. (1986). The Lowe and Costello Symbolic Play Test in socially impaired children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 199–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haight, W., & Miller, P. J. (1992). The development of everyday pretend play: A longitudinal study of mothers’ participation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 331–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 498–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Executive function deficits and the pretend play of children with autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 1473–1482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. K. (1996). Generativity defects in pretend play in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 275–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1971). Follow-up study of eleven autistic children originally reported in 1943. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 119–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1992). Follow-up study of eleven autistic children originally reported in 1943. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 7, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, P., Leckman, J., & Ort, S. (1989). Delayed response performance in males with Fragile X. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 39, 951–962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 325–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C. et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1989). Stability of assessment results of autistic and non-autistic language-impaired children from preschool years to early school age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 30, 575–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotter, V. (1978). Follow-up studies. In M. Rutter, & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 475–495). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, D. M., & Langone, J. (1998). Variability in the play of preschoolers with cognitive delays across different toy sets. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 15, 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCune, L., Dipane, D., Fireoved, R., & Fleck, M. (1994). Play: A context for mutual regulation within mother-child interaction. In A. Slade, & D. P. Wolff (Eds.), Children at play: Clinical and developmental approaches to meaning and representation (pp. 148–166). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development, 52, 785–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, R. E., Roger, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 34, 563–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, B., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak central coherence, poor joint attention, and low verbal ability: Independent deficits in early autism. Developmental Psychology, 39, 646–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyles, J. R. (1994). The excellence of play. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning Cranston, RI: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1996). A preliminary manual for the abridged Early Social Communication Scales. Miami, FL, USA: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Neal, R. A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (pp. 139–168). San Diego, CA, USA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Roger, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081–1105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B., Rogers, S., Bennetto, L., Griffith, E. M., Reed, D. T., & Shyu, V. (1997). Validity tests of the executive dysfunction hypothesis of autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 143–178). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York, USA: W.W. Norton and Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riguet, C. B., Taylor, N. D., Benaroya, S., & Klein, L. S. (1981). Symbolic play in autistic, Down’s, and normal children of equivalent mental age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 439–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., & Stiano, T. (1999). Social-cognitive development in the first year. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T. & Wehner, E. (2003) Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44 (5), 763–781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russ, S. W., Robins, A. L., & Christiano, B. A. (1999). Pretend play: Longitudinal prediction of creativity and affect in fantasy in children. Creative Research Journal, 12, 129–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The “windows task” as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). The cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 289–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 39, 817–827.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, v-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1976). The psychology of play. New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind and Language, 18, 121–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1992). A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, 489–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45, 135–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S. R., & Brierley, L. M. (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally retarded and in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 18, 167–178.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

SJR and SH were partially supported by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant #PO1HD35468. SJR was also supported by National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders NIDCD Grant #R21 DC05574. The support of the Developmental Psychobiology Research Group and the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism are gratefully acknowledged.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to M. D. Rutherford.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rutherford, M.D., Young, G.S., Hepburn, S. et al. A Longitudinal Study of Pretend Play in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 1024–1039 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0240-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0240-9

Keywords

Navigation