Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Reading Comprehension in Boys with ADHD: The Mediating Roles of Working Memory and Orthographic Conversion

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reading comprehension difficulties in children with ADHD are well established; however, limited information exists concerning the cognitive mechanisms that contribute to these difficulties and the extent to which they interact with one another. The current study examines two broad cognitive processes known to be involved in children’s reading comprehension abilities—(a) working memory (i.e., central executive processes [CE], phonological short-term memory [PH STM], and visuospatial short-term memory [VS STM]) and (b) orthographic conversion (i.e., conversion of visually presented text to a phonological code)—to elucidate their unique and interactive contribution to ADHD-related reading comprehension differences. Thirty-one boys with ADHD-combined type and 30 typically developing (TD) boys aged 8 to 12 years (M = 9.64, SD = 1.22) were administered multiple counterbalanced tasks assessing WM and orthographic conversion processes. Relative to TD boys, boys with ADHD exhibited significant deficits in PH STM (d = −0.70), VS STM (d = −0.92), CE (d = −1.58), and orthographic conversion (d = −0.93). Bias-corrected, bootstrapped mediation analyses revealed that CE and orthographic conversion processes modeled separately mediated ADHD-related reading comprehension differences partially, whereas PH STM and VS STM did not. CE and orthographic conversion modeled jointly mediated ADHD-related reading comprehension differences fully wherein orthographic conversion’s large magnitude influence on reading comprehension occurred indirectly through CE’s impact on the orthographic system. The findings suggest that adaptive cognitive interventions designed to improve reading-related outcomes in children with ADHD may benefit by including modules that train CE and orthographic conversion processes independently and interactively.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Studies by Rosen and Engle (1997) and others (e.g., Colom et al. 2005; Swanson and Kim 2007) provide compelling evidence that forward and backward simple digit span tasks load on a PH STM factor and are statistically separable from PH WM measures such as complex span tasks, the latter of which are more highly correlated with measures of children’s reading competence.

  2. All participants met criteria for ADHD-Combined Type using DSM-5 diagnostic criteria.

  3. Scores for one TD child exceeded 1.5 SDs on one of the two parents’ but not teachers’ rating scales. Parent interview revealed no significant ADHD symptoms or symptoms associated with other clinical disorders for the child. Seven children with ADHD had subthreshold scores on teacher-rated hyperactivity/impulsivity. Follow-up clinical interviews, however, indicated the subthreshold symptoms were attributable to substantial psychostimulant effects while they were rated, and that all children demonstrated a history of significant, persistent levels of hyperactivity/impulsivity both at home and at school.

  4. PH WM and VS WM performance data for a subset of the current sample were used in separate studies to evaluate conceptually unrelated hypotheses (Alderson et al. 2010, 2012; Kofler et al. 2010, 2011, 2014; Raiker et al. 2012; Rapport et al. 2008a, 2009a; Sarver et al. 2015). We have not previously reported the reading comprehension or orthographic speed/accuracy data or their associations with our WM tasks for any children in the current sample.

  5. The N-to-K ratio of 61:2 was within recommended guidelines for deriving the Orthographic Conversion variable (Hogarty et al. 2005).

  6. Age was examined as a potential covariate given its trend towards significance and was a significant covariate for two of the mediators (CE and Orthographic Conversion) but not a significant covariate for any of the model’s dependent variables. The marginal covariate effects did not affect the pattern or interpretation of results.

  7. Other alternative approaches, such as using a GAI, were considered but not adopted because the GAI shares considerable variance with WM (i.e. r = 0.50 to 0.63) and because VCI and PRI factor scores also share considerable variance with WM.

  8. Briefly, the wider 95 % confidence interval increases the likelihood that the confidence interval for c′ will include 0.0, indicating that diagnostic status and reading comprehension are no longer related significantly after accounting for the mediator (i.e., full mediation). In contrast, the narrower 90 % confidence interval is less likely to include 0.0, and therefore is likely to result in a more conservative conclusion regarding the magnitude of the relation between diagnostic status and reading comprehension after accounting for the mediator (i.e., partial mediation). For discussion and specific examples of this phenomenon, see Shrout and Bolger (2002).

  9. Contemporary use of the terms near transfer and far transfer effects refers to an increase in performance on tasks that are highly similar and dissimilar to those used during training, respectively.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alderson, R. M., Rapport, M. D., Hudec, K. L., Sarver, D. E., & Kofler, M. J. (2010). Competing core processes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): do working memory deficiencies underlie behavioral inhibition deficits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alderson, R. M., Rapport, M. D., Kasper, L. J., Sarver, D. E., & Kofler, M. J. (2012). Hyperactivity in boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the association between deficient behavioral inhibition, attentional processes, and objectively measured activity. Child Neuropsychology, 18, 487–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bálint, S., Czobor, P., Komlosi, S., Meszaros, A., Simon, V., & Bitter, I. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): gender-and age-related differences in neurocognition. Psychological Medicine, 39(8), 1337–1345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (2007). School interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: where to from here? School Psychology Review, 36, 279–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, K. J., Brown, K. S., Boyle, M., Racine, Y., & Offord, D. (2003). Does low reading achievement at school entry cause conduct problems? Social Science & Medicine, 56, 2443–2448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, S. E., & Knapp, P. K. (1996). Reading comprehension abilities of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cedrus Corporation (2002). SuperLab Pro (Version 2) [Computer Software]. San Pedro, CA: Cedrus Corportation.

  • Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Rebollo, I., & Shih, P. C. (2005). Memory span and general intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Intelligence, 33, 623–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 331–343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dirlikov, B., Rosch, K. S., Crocetti, D., Denckla, M. B., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Distinct frontal lobe morphology in girls and boys with ADHD. Neuroimage: Clinical, 7, 222–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DuPaul, G. J., Gormley, M. J., & Laracy, S. D. (2013). Comorbidity of LD and ADHD: implications of DSM-5 for assessment and treatment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, 43–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, J.N., Langberg, J.M., Rosen, P.J., Graham, A., Narad, M.E., Antonini, T.N., … & Altaye, M. (2011). Evidence for higher reaction time variability for children with ADHD on a range of cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology, 25, 427–441.

  • Finn, E.S., Shen, X., Holahan, J.M., Scheinost, D., Lacadie, C., Papademetris, X., … & Constable, R.T. (2014). Disruption of functional networks in dyslexia: A whole-brain, data-driven analysis of connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 397–404.

  • Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and achievement meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18, 233–239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gadow, K., Sprafkin, J., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J., & Loney, J. (2004). Further validity evidence for the teacher version of the Child Symptom Inventory-4. School Psychology Quarterly, 19, 50–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1036–1045.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gremillion, M. L., & Martel, M. M. (2012). Semantic language as a mechanism explaining the association between ADHD symptoms and reading and mathematics underachievement. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1339–1349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guttentag, R. E., & Haith, M. M. (1978). Automatic processing as a function of age and reading ability. Child Development, 49, 707–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B., & Fossati, P. (2004). Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38, 567–576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A.F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

  • Hayes, A. F. (2014). PROCESS for SPSS (Version 2.12.1) [Computer Software]. Columbus, OH.

  • Hogarty, K. Y., Hines, C. V., Kromrey, J. D., Ferron, J. M., & Mumford, K. R. (2005). The quality of factor solutions in exploratory factor analysis: the influence of sample size, communality, and overdetermination. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65, 202–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johns, J. L. (1988). Basic reading inventory: Pre-primer through grade twelve and early literacy (4th ed.). Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasper, L. J., Alderson, R. M., & Hudec, K. L. (2012). Moderators of working memory deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 605–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1998). Manual for the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement Normative Update (KTEA-I-NU). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Manual for the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement Second Edition (KTEA-II). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980988.

  • Kintsch, W., & Rawson, K. A. (2007). Comprehension. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: a handbook (pp. 209–226). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Sarver, D. E., & Raiker, J. S. (2010). ADHD and working memory: the impact of central executive deficits and exceeding storage/rehearsal capacity on observed inattentive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 149–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., & Alderson, R. M. (2011). Working memory deficits and social problems in children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 805–817.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Alderson, R. M., Raiker, J. S., Bolden, J., Sarver, D. E., & Rapport, M. D. (2014). Working memory and intraindividual variability as neurocognitive indicators in ADHD: Examining competing model predictions. Neuropsychology, 28, 459–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 643–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, K. E., Lazar, N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., & Sweeney, J. A. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., & Mackenzie, G. (2015). Reading comprehension in adolescents with ADHD: Exploring the poor comprehender profile and individual differences in vocabulary and executive functions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 38, 329–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maughan, B., Gray, G., & Rutter, M. (1985). Reading retardation and antisocial behaviour: a follow-up into employment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26(5), 741–758.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D., Bell, L. C., France, I. M., & Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Phoneme-specific interference in reading: the tongue-twister effect revisited. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGee, R., Prior, M., Williams, S., Smart, D., & Sanson, A. (2002). The long-term significance of teacher-rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: findings from two longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 1004–1017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8(4), 463–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Childhood executive function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood-diagnosed ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 657–668.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. C., Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2013). Reading comprehension in children with ADHD: cognitive underpinnings of the centrality deficit. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 473–483.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, C., Goldstein, D. E., Nourse, S., & Edgar, E. (2000). The postsecondary school attendance and completion rates of high school graduates with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 119–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Shaughnessy, T. E., & Swanson, H. L. (1998). Do immediate memory deficits in students with learning disabilities in reading reflect a developmental lag or deficit? A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Learning Disability Quarterly, 21, 123–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 29, 344–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E., Foster, E. M., & Robb, J. A. (2007). The economic impact of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 7, 121–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J. (2007). The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: a handbook (pp. 227–247). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pham, A. V., & Hasson, R. M. (2014). Verbal and visuospatial working memory as predictors of children’s reading ability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29, 467–477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Power, T. J. (1992). Contextual factors in vigilance testing of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 579–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raiker, J. S., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., & Sarver, D. E. (2012). Objectively-measured impulsivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): testing competing predictions from the working memory and behavioral inhibition models of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 699–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Alderson, R. M., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Bolden, J., & Sims, V. (2008a). Working memory deficits in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the contribution of central executive and subsystem processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 825–837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., Alderson, R. M., & Raiker, J. S. (2008b). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder. In M. Hersen & D. Reitman (Eds.), Handbook of psychological assessment, case conceptualization, and treatment, Vol 2: children and adolescents (pp. 349–404). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., & Alderson, R. M. (2009a). Hyperactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a ubiquitous core symptom or manifestation of working memory deficits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 521–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., Alderson, R. M., Timko, T., Jr., & DuPaul, G. J. (2009b). Variability of attention processes in ADHD: observations from the classroom. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 563–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., & Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 1237–1252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, M., Hwang, H., Toplak, M., Weiss, M., & Tannock, R. (2011). Inattention, working memory, and academic achievement in adolescents referred for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 17, 444–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R. W. (1997). Forward and backward serial recall. Intelligence, 25, 37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarver, D. E., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., Raiker, J. S., & Friedman, L. M. (2015). Hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): impairing deficit or compensatory behavior? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1219–1232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., Lavers, N., & Pillay, V. (2007). Working memory and reading difficulties: what we know and what we don’t know about the relationship. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 185–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Working memory capacity and the scope and control of attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P., & Shalev, L. (2013). The role of sustained attention and display medium in reading comprehension among adolescents with ADHD and without it. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 431–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Alloway, T. P. (2012). Working memory, learning, and academic achievement. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook, Vol. 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 327–366). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Howell, M. (2001). Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictors of children’s reading performance at different ages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 720–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 249–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35, 151–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 294–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Ickowicz, A., & Schachar, R. (1995). Differential effects of methylphenidate on working memory in ADHD children with and without comorbid anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 886–896.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Titz, C., & Karbach, J. (2014). Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychological Research, 78, 852–868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. J., & Marois, R. (2005). Posterior parietal cortex activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term memory capacity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 144–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau (2013). Current Population Survey. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/hhes/school/data/cps/2013/tables.html.

  • Ville Junod, R. E., DuPaul, G. J., Jitendra, A. K., Volpe, R. J., & Cleary, K. S. (2006). Classroom observations of students with and without ADHD: differences across types of engagement. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 87–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, D., & Johnston, C. (2015). Gender differences in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a narrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 15–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark D. Rapport.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Funding

This study was conducted without external funding.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent/assent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Friedman, L.M., Rapport, M.D., Raiker, J.S. et al. Reading Comprehension in Boys with ADHD: The Mediating Roles of Working Memory and Orthographic Conversion. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45, 273–287 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0171-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0171-7

Keywords

Navigation