Skip to main content
Log in

Anxiety and Attentional Bias in Preschool-Aged Children: An Eyetracking Study

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Extensive research has examined attentional bias for threat in anxious adults and school-aged children but it is unclear when this anxiety-related bias is first established. This study uses eyetracking technology to assess attentional bias in a sample of 83 children aged 3 or 4 years. Of these, 37 (19 female) met criteria for an anxiety disorder and 46 (30 female) did not. Gaze was recorded during a free-viewing task with angry-neutral face pairs presented for 1250 ms. There was no indication of between-group differences in threat bias, with both anxious and non-anxious groups showing vigilance for angry faces as well as longer dwell times to angry over neutral faces. Importantly, however, the anxious participants spent significantly less time looking at the faces overall, when compared to the non-anxious group. The results suggest that both anxious and non-anxious preschool-aged children preferentially attend to threat but that anxious children may be more avoidant of faces than non-anxious children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Prior to conducting any interviews for the present research, all interviewers first watched at least two videos of ADIS interviews. For each, they were asked to assign diagnoses and CSRs. These were then discussed with a senior clinical psychologist, with reference to the diagnoses and CSRs given by the original interviewer. Once the clinical psychologist was satisfied with their performance, trainees observed two ADIS interviews in our clinic. Again they were asked to assign diagnoses and CSRs and these were discussed after the interview with the psychologist who had conducted the assessment. Once they had completed at least two of these and assigned diagnoses and CSRs that were equivalent to those made by the interviewing clinical psychologist, they were then observed conducting at least two interviews. Again, assigned diagnoses and CSRs were discussed with the psychologist who had observed the interview. The clinical psychologist who oversaw the training made a decision regarding the interviewers readiness to conduct interviews for the study based on their overall performance on these tasks. Note that this procedure is followed for training psychologists in our clinic, where excellent reliability is reported (Lyneham, Abbott, and Rapee 2007). Interviewers attended regular supervision sessions with a senior clinical psychologist where difficult diagnostic cases were discussed.

  2. Following a request from an anonymous reviewer, we examined whether the results were driven by participants with a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder. The significant between-group effects reported above for length of first fixation and dwell time were examined comparing three groups of participants: those with a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder anywhere in their diagnostic profile (n = 22), clinically anxious participants without a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (n = 15) and participants with no anxiety diagnosis (n = 44). These analyses also showed no group by emotion interactions and a significant main effect of group for both length of first fixation, F(2, 80) = 3.93, p = 0.023, and dwell time, F(2, 80) = 6.08, p = 0.003. Follow-up t-tests comparing groups showed that the group with social anxiety disorder had shorter first fixations to faces and shorter dwell time to faces than the control comparison group but neither reached significance (p > 0.08). The clinically anxious group without social anxiety disorder also had shorter first fixations to faces and shorter dwell time to faces than the control comparison group and both reached significance (p < 0.005). The two anxious groups were not significantly different from each other on either dependent variable (p > 0.09).

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, T., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 704–723. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, K. E., McNally, R. J., Rapee, R. M., Gamble, A. L., & Wiseman, A. L. (2007). Processing of emotional faces in children and adolescents with anxiety disorders. Behaviour Change, 24, 183–194. doi:10.1375/bech.24.4.183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, H. M., Eley, T. C., Broeren, S., MacLeod, C., Rinck, M., Hadwin, J. A., & Lester, K. J. (2014). Psychometric properties of reaction time based experimental paradigms measuring anxiety-related information-processing biases in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 97–107. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., & Fox, N. A. (2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 928–935. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, E., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837–844. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 313–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 955–960. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. P., & Lester, K. J. (2010). Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13, 315–332. doi:10.1007/s10567-010-0078-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, A. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2009). The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 841–847. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019. doi:10.2307/1130152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, M. (2010). Assessment of attentional bias using the dot-probe task in anxious children and adolescents. In J. H. A. P. Field (Ed.), Information processing biases and anxiety: A developmental perspective (pp. 77–109). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, A. S., Carrion, V., Kletter, H., Karchemskiy, A., Weems, C. F., & Reiss, A. (2012). Brain activation to facial expressions in youth with PTSD symptoms. Depression and Anxiety, 29, 449–459. doi:10.1002/da.21892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Masek, B., Henin, A., Blakely, L. R., Pollock-Wurman, R. A., McQuade, J., & Biederman, J. (2010). Cognitive behavioral therapy for 4- to 7-year-old children with anxiety disorders: a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 498–510. doi:10.1037/a0019055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. L., & Dodd, H. F. (2012). Informing early intervention: preschool predictors of anxiety disorders in middle childhood. PLoS ONE, 7, e42359. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042359.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huijding, J., Mayer, B., Koster, E. H., & Muris, P. (2011). To look or not to look: an eyemovement study of hypervigilance during change detection in high and low spider fearful students. Emotion, 11, 666–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • In-Albon, T., Kossowsky, J., & Schneider, S. (2010). Vigilance and avoidance of threat in the eye movements of children with separation anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 225–235. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9359-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kindt, M., Bierman, D., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the stroop task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 163–179. doi:10.1006/jecp.1997.2376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2005). Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1087–1098. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1757–1771. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H. J., Hawk, S. T., & van Knippenberg, A. (2010). Presentation and validation of the radboud faces database. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1377–1388. doi:10.1080/02699930903485076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2014). Sensitivity to social and non-social threats in temperamentally shy children at-risk for anxiety. Developmental Science, 17, 239–247. doi:10.1111/desc.12110.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyneham, H. J., Abbott, M. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Interrater reliability of the anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV: child and parent version. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 731–736. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e3180465a09.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in Emotional Disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacNamara, A., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Distinct electrocortical and behavioral evidence for increased attention to threat in generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 234–243. doi:10.1002/da.20679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.000325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809–848. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967%2898%2900063-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Miles, F., & Dixon, R. (2004). Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 689–700. doi:10.1080/02699930341000158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Reactivity to feaful expressions of familiar and unfamiliar children with autism: an eye-tracking pupillometry study. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6, 14–30. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-6-14.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 381–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perez-Edgar, K., McDermott, J. N., Korelitz, K., Degnan, K. A., Curby, T. W., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Patterns of sustained attention in infancy shape the developmental trajectory of social behavior from toddlerhood through adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1723–1730. doi:10.1037/a0021064.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perez-Edgar, K., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., McDermott, J. M., White, L. K., Henderson, H. A., Degnan, K. A., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 885–895. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9495-5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S., Ingram, M., Edwards, S., & Sweeney, L. (2005). Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 488–497. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1986). Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 211–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanson, A., Smart, D., Prior, M., Oberklaid, F., & Pedlow, R. (1994). The structure of temperament from age 3 to 7 years: age, sex, and sociodemographic influences. Merrill-Palmer quarterly, 40, 233–252. http://www.asu.edu/clas/ssfd/mpq/.

  • Seefeldt, W. L., Kramer, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Heinrichs, N. (2014). Hypervigilance and avoidance in visual attention in children with social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 105–112. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shechner, T., Britton, J. C., Pérez-Edgar, K., Bar-Haim, Y., Ernst, M., Fox, N. A., & Pine, S. D. (2012). Attention biases, anxiety, and development: toward or away from threats or rewards? Depression and Anxiety, 29, 282–284. doi:10.1002/da.20914.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shechner, T., Jarcho, J. M., Britton, J. C., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2013). Attention bias of anxious youth during extended exposure of emotional face pairs: an eyetracking study. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 14–21. doi:10.1002/da.21986.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). The anxiety disorders interview schedule for children for DSM-IV: child and parent versions. San Antonia: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stirling, L. J., Eley, T. C., & Clark, D. M. (2006). Preliminary evidence for an association between social anxiety symptoms and avoidance of negative faces in school-age children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 431–439. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taghavi, M. R., Dalgleish, T., Moradi, A. R., Neshat-Doost, H. T., & Yule, W. (2003). Selective processing of negative emotional information in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 221–230. doi:10.1348/01446650360703348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, A. M., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2013). Biased attention to threat in paediatric anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, specific phobia, separation anxiety disorder) as a function of ‘distress’ versus ‘fear’ diagnostic categorization. Psychological Medicine, firstview, 1–10. doi:10.1017/S0033291713000779.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, S. E., & Weems, C. F. (2006). Associations among selective attention, memory bias, cognitive errors and symptoms of anxiety in youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 838–849. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9066-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 985–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted as part of an Australian Research Council funded project (DP0878609 & DP0342793) held by Professor Hudson. Thank you to the interns and students who helped with stimuli, data collection and coding for this project, including Laurie Monier, Anni Kuusik, and Irma Knuistingh. Thanks also to Emily Connaughton and Eugene McSorely for their help with the eye-tracking data, Suzannah Ravenscroft for her comments on an earlier draft and to the families who participated.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helen F. Dodd.

Additional information

This research was conducted at the Centre for Emotional Health, Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dodd, H.F., Hudson, J.L., Williams, T. et al. Anxiety and Attentional Bias in Preschool-Aged Children: An Eyetracking Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol 43, 1055–1065 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9962-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9962-x

Keywords

Navigation