Skip to main content
Log in

Episodes to scripts to rules: concrete-abstractions in kindergarten children’s explanations of a robot’s behavior

  • Published:
International Journal of Technology and Design Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explores young children’s abstraction of the rules underlying a robot’s emergent behavior. The study was conducted individually with six kindergarten children, along five sessions that included description and construction tasks, ordered by increasing difficulty. We developed and used a robotic control interface, structured as independent concurrent rules. To capture the children’s changing knowledge representations, we have employed a framework that underscores the differences in generality between episodes, a unique sequence of events, scripts, which include repeating temporal patterns, triggered by an environmental condition and rules, atemporal associations between local environmental conditions and the robot’s actions. Our data unravels the progression through which rules are constructed. From an episode that focuses on the robot’s actions, noticing repeated sequences triggered by occasional environmental conditions emerges into scripts. Once both actions and conditions are attributed with similar importance, noticing the co-variance of environmental conditions with robot actions is made possible, bolstering abstraction of atemporal rules. In addition, we have supported the children’s reasoning by helping them attend to relevant features, and compared their spontaneous and supported descriptions. We elaborate on the role of function and mechanism as invariants, and the support of “concrete-abstractions” in the interaction between cognitive schemas and object-embedded abstract schemas, for the children’s evolving explanations of the robot’s behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. While in algorithmic programming languages, such as flow-charts, rules can contain scripts and scripts can contain rules, the environment we have constructed is based on a different control paradigm, which does not include scripts (e.g. the finite state machine, and the use of ladder diagrams; see Mioduser et al. 1996).

  2. One may conceive of this process as regression in the zone of proximal development. When the task is too complex, the cognitive load is great and one reverts to earlier ways of thinking. We thank Sidney Strauss, Tel-Aviv University, for helping us in this interpretation.

References

  • Barton, M. E., & Komatsu, L. K. (1989). Defining features of natural kinds and artifacts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(5), 433–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Yam, Y. (1997). Dynamics of complex systems. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, The Advanced Book Program.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bers, M. U., & Portsmore, M. (2005). Teaching partnerships: Early childhood and engineering student teaching math and science through robotics. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14(1), 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betzer, N. (2002). The knowledge constructing process and the design process constructing among high achiever pupils through Project-Based Learning (PBL). Dissertation. Tel-Aviv University.

  • Braitenberg, V. (1984). Vehicles: Experiments in synthetic psychology. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18, 32–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, M. (2000). Technological affordance, social practice and learning narratives in early childhood setting. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 10, 61–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, P. W. (1997). From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. Psychological Review, 104, 367–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (1993). Cognitive development. 3rd ed. NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (1999). The science of technology: Young children working technologically. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 9, 269–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (2000). Working technologically: Investigations into how young children design and make during technology education. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 10, 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., Brooks, P. J., & Samuels, M. C. (1996). Inference and action in early causal reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 120–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, M., Kitano, H., & Doi, T. T. (2000). Robot entertainment. In A. Druin & J. Hendler (Eds.), Robots for kids: Exploring new technologies for learning (pp. 37–72). San-Francisco, California: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (1978). A study of early word meaning using artificial objects: What looks like a jiggy but acts like a zimbo? Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granott, N. (1991a). Puzzled minds and weird creatures: Phases in the spontaneous process of knowledge construction. In I. Harel & S. Papert (Eds.), Constructionism (pp. 295–310). Norwood, New-Jersey: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granott, N. (1991b). From macro to micro and back: On the analysis of microdevelopment. Paper presented at the meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, PA.

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Pfeffer, M. G. (2004). Comparing expert and novice understanding of a complex system from the perspective of structures, behaviors, and functions. Cognitive Science, 28, 127–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoyles, C., Noss, R., Adamson, R. & Lowe, S. (2001). Programming rules: What do children understand? In Proceedings of the 25rd. Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol. 3, pp. 169–176, Utrecht.

  • Jacobson, M. J. (2001). Problem solving, cognition, and complex systems: Differences between experts and novices. Complexity, 6(3), 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, T., & Rennie, L. J. (1998). Factors that influence children’s developing perceptions of technology. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 8, 261–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, K. (1996). ToonTalk™ – An animated programming environment for children. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 7, 197–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • KemlerNelson, D. G. & 11 Swarthmore College Students (1995). Principle-based inferences in young children’s categorization: Revisiting the impact of function on the naming of artifacts. Cognitive development, 10, 347–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., Fay, A. L., & Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 111–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Dean, D. Jr. (2004). Connecting scientific reasoning and causal inference. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5(2), 261–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy S. T., & Mioduser, D. (2007). Does it “want” or “was it programmed to...”? Kindergarten children’s explanations of an autonomous robot’s adaptive functioning. Accepted for publication in the International Journal of Technology and Design Education.

  • Maddocks, R. (2000). Bolts from the blue: How large dreams can become real products. In A. Druin & J. Hendler (Eds.) Robots for kids: Exploring new technologies for learning (pp. 111–156). San-Francisco, California: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markovits, H. (2002). The development of conditional reasoning: A mental model account. Developmental Review, 22, 5–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mioduser, D., Venezky, R. L., & Gong, B. (1996). Student’s perception and design of simple control systems. Computers in Human Behavior, 12(3), 363–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montemayor, J., Druin, A, & Hendler, J. (2000). PETS: A personal electronic teller of stories. In A. Druin & J. Hendler (Eds.), Robots for kids: Exploring new technologies for learning (pp. 73–110). San-Francisco, California: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgado, L., Cruz, M. G. B., & Kahn, K. (2001). Working in ToonTalk with 4- and 5-year olds. Paper presented at the Playground International Seminar, April 2001, Porto, Portugal.

  • Muller, U., Overton, W. F., & Reene, K. (2001). Development of conditional reasoning: A longitudinal study. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(1), 27–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niazzi, T., & Gopnik, A. (2003). Sorting and acting with objects in early childhood: An exploration of the use of causal cues. Cognitive Development, 18, 299–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W., Byrnes, J. P., & O’Brien, D. P. (1985). Developmental and individual differences in conditional reasoning: The role of contradiction training and cognitive style. Developmental Psychology, 21(4), 692–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papert, S. (1980, 1993). Mindstorms: children, computers, and powerful ideas. 1st and 2nd eds. Cambridge, MA: Basic Books.

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1948/1956). The child’s conception of space. New-York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1972). Explanations of machines. In The child’s conception of physical causality (pp. 195–236). New-Jersey: Littlefield Adams & Co.

  • Resnick, M. (1998). Technologies for lifelong kindergarten. Educational Technology Research & Development. 46(4), 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, M., Martin, F., Sargent, R., & Silverman, B. (1996). Programmable bricks: Toys to think with. IBM Systems Journal, 35(3–4), 443–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, New-Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L., & Black, J. B. (1996). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20, 457–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R. (1995). Is human learning rational? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A(2), 257–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1986). Children’s thinking. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (1998). Developmental differences in rule learning: A microgenetic analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 273–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D., & Keil, F. (1995). An abstract to concrete shift in the development of biological thought: The insides story. Cognition, 56, 129–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, D. M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gopnik, A. (2004). Children’s causal inferences from indirect evidence: Backwards blocking and Bayesian reasoning in preschoolers. Cognitive Science, 28, 303–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talis, V., Levy, S. T., & Mioduser, D. (1998). RoboGAN: Interface for programming a robot with rules for young children. Tel-Aviv: Tel-Aviv University.

  • van Duuren, M., Dossett, B., & Robinson, D. (1998). Gauging children’s understanding of artificially intelligent objects: A presentation of “counterfactuals. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22(4), 871–889.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilensky, U., & Resnick, M. (1999). Thinking in levels: A dynamic systems perspective to making sense of the world. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 8(1), 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyeth, P., & Purchase, H. C. (2000). Programming without a computer: A new interface for children under eight. Paper presented at the User Interface Conference, AUIC, January–February, 2000. First Australasian.

  • Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning skills. Developmental Review, 20, 99–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuga, K. F. (2004). Improving technology education research on cognition. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 14, 79–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Ms. Diana Levy, a graduate student in the Knowledge Technology Lab at Tel-Aviv University, for her assistance in coding the data and performing the initial analysis.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Mioduser.

Appendix I: Coded transcription

Appendix I: Coded transcription

   The following are transcripts of two conversations with Naomi, as she describes the robot “Guarding an island” and two weeks later as “The cat in the hat likes black”. The coding according to the variables is included

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mioduser, D., Levy, S.T. & Talis, V. Episodes to scripts to rules: concrete-abstractions in kindergarten children’s explanations of a robot’s behavior. Int J Technol Des Educ 19, 15–36 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10798-007-9040-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10798-007-9040-6

Keywords

Navigation