1 General Introduction

A great deal of research has been devoted to the understanding of causes—in other words, underlying processes—of gender differences in negotiation (e.g., Kray et al. 2001; Stuhlmacher and Linnabery 2013). Past research has documented processes that hinder women, in particular, from negotiating effectively (e.g., Amanatullah and Morris 2010; Bowles et al. 2007). Yet, recent empirical (Kray and Haselhuhn 2012; Netchaeva et al. 2015) and theoretical (Kennedy and Kray 2015; Mazei et al. 2021; Miller 2013) advances suggest that processes that concern women may not be the full story: men are often anxious about not appearing “man enough” (Gilmore 1990; Vandello and Bosson 2013) and, hence, become especially competitive in negotiations (Netchaeva et al. 2015). However, research tends to focus either on processes that concern women or, by contrast, on processes that concern men (see Kennedy and Kray 2015, or Kray et al. 2001, for examples of exceptions). In this way, a comprehensive understanding of gender differences in negotiation is lacking, and researchers are left unguided as to whether processes that concern men represent a particularly worthwhile research avenue, especially in comparison to processes that concern women. Thus, we explored people’s beliefs regarding processes concerning both men and women in negotiation.

Gaining insight into people’s beliefs, which capture what people think is true (Gilbert 1991; Markóczy 1997), is relevant for two reasons: first, beliefs can be an important source of information as they are often based on actual events. For example, beliefs about the characteristics of groups result from observations of their actual activities in society (Eagly and Steffen 1984; Koenig and Eagly 2014). Second, beliefs influence “perceptions, interpretations, and reactions, and in so doing create ‘self-fulfilling prophecies’” (Ross et al. 2010, p. 4; see also Manea et al. 2020). Altogether, beliefs are relevant given their informational value concerning the causes of gender differences and their practical consequences.

Hence, we provide insight into people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation. Hereby, we increase the understanding of processes that concern men specifically by beginning a line of inquiry concerning their role in the emergence of gender differences (see also Kennedy and Kray 2015; Netchaeva et al. 2015), relative to processes that concern women (e.g., Stuhlmacher and Linnabery 2013). Moreover, we explore whether men and women have different beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiations (see also Bosson et al. 2021). Certain processes, such as discrimination, can be subtle (Jones et al. 2016), so that people who are not subject to such a process (e.g., men) may overlook them (Swim et al. 2001; Weaver et al. 2010). Thus, examining whether women and men differ in their beliefs provides insight into the antecedents of people’s beliefs (e.g., being oneself subject to a certain process; but see Bosson et al. 2021). Finally, we examine the potential consequences of these beliefs by examining their relationships with people’s appraisals of different diversity initiatives and their willingness to support them (see also Manea et al. 2020). Thus, our findings also have practical implications for promoting diversity goals in organizations (e.g., Leslie 2019).

1.1 Processes Concerning Women

Women often negotiate less frequently (Kugler et al. 2018; Small et al. 2007) and less successfully than men (Bowles et al. 2005; Mazei et al. 2015). These gender differences are typically explained (e.g., Bear and Babcock 2017; Kugler et al. 2018) with social role theory (Eagly 1987): people observe that men and women often engage in different activities—for example, women are more likely to take care of children (Croft et al. 2015; Eagly and Carli 2007). As a result, people believe that women are more communal (e.g., cooperative) but less agentic (e.g., competitive) than men (Eagly et al. 2020; Eagly and Steffen 1984; Rudman et al. 2012). These beliefs are captured in the concept of gender roles: “the collection of both descriptive and injunctive expectations associated with women and men” (Eagly and Karau 2002, p. 574). The injunctive component can be further divided into prescriptions and proscriptions (“how men and women should behave” and “should not behave”; Rudman et al. 2012, p. 166).

Gender roles matter because people often internalize them (Eagly and Wood 2012; Wood and Eagly 2010), and, as a result, exhibit behaviors that are in line with their gender role (Croft et al. 2015; Twenge 1997). Moreover, others react negatively to women and men who deviate from their gender roles (Amanatullah and Tinsley 2013; Heilman and Wallen 2010), a reaction known as backlash (Rudman 1998). As such, people typically behave in ways consistent with their gender role to avoid backlash (Amanatullah and Morris 2010; Rudman and Fairchild 2004).

Women can be disadvantaged in negotiations because agentic behaviors increase economic outcomes (Hüffmeier et al. 2014; Kulik and Olekalns 2012). However, when women show agentic negotiation behaviors, they risk incurring backlash (Amanatullah and Tinsley 2013; Bowles et al. 2007). Altogether, there is an incongruity (Eagly and Karau 2002) between women’s communal gender role and the agency of an economically successful negotiator (e.g., Kugler et al. 2018; Stuhlmacher and Linnabery 2013). Still, women certainly are no less effective negotiators per se (Bowles et al. 2005; Kray et al. 2001): for instance, women negotiate more ethically than men (e.g., Kray and Haselhuhn 2012), which is likely to enhance trust (cf. Kennedy and Kray 2015; Lee et al. 2017) and, hence, achieve joint gains in integrative negotiations (Kong et al. 2014; but see Calhoun and Smith 1999). Women can also use strategies to overcome their disadvantages (e.g., Bear and Babcock 2017; Bowles and Babcock 2013) or may even use their femininity to their advantage (Kray et al. 2012). In sum, negotiation research has illuminated processes that concern women (Haselhuhn and Kray 2012; Miller 2013), which is clearly valuable. Yet, explaining gender differences only by processes that concern women also has limitations: in 2 meta-analyses that used role incongruity among women as a theoretical lens (Kugler et al. 2018; Mazei et al. 2015), much variability in gender differences was left unaccounted for (Lee et al. 2017; Mazei et al. 2021).

1.2 Processes Concerning Men

Processes that concern men may also help to explain gender effects in negotiation (e.g., Cantrell and Butler 1997; Kennedy and Kray 2015; Miller 2013). For example, Netchaeva et al. (2015) found that men, but not women, experienced a threat and behaved particularly assertively when they negotiated with a female, but not a male, hiring manager. Moreover, Kray and Haselhuhn (2012) found that processes that concern men (i.e., the perception that negotiating is a masculine activity) help to account for gender differences in the usage of unethical negotiation tactics (for further relevant work, see Haselhuhn et al. 2014; Kray et al. 2001; Lee et al. 2017).

This aforementioned negotiation research is typically grounded in the precarious manhood thesis (Vandello and Bosson 2013; Vandello et al. 2008), which argues that men often feel the need to show that they are “real” men (Gilmore 1990). In turn, being considered a “real” man requires confirmation by other people (Bosson and Vandello 2011; Vandello et al. 2008). Men’s status as a “real” man is also easily questioned, so that men frequently experience anxiety (Vandello and Bosson 2013; Vandello et al. 2008) and aim to “prove” their manhood (e.g., Berke et al. 2017; Bosson et al. 2009). Mazei et al. (2021) integrated these insights into a model concerning the negotiation context, which proposes that men perceive their negotiation performance as affecting their status as a “real” man (Kennedy and Kray 2015; Kray and Haselhuhn 2012), which then shapes their negotiation behaviors (Miller 2013; Netchaeva et al. 2015). Many propositions of their model, however, still await empirical testing.

1.3 The Current Research

Extant work typically focuses either on processes that concern women or on processes that concern men—that is, the two approaches remained disconnected (but see Kennedy and Kray 2015; Kray et al. 2001). This is problematic as there remains uncertainty regarding the relative importance of the different processes: although processes concerning men may in principle exist, it could be that they play only a minor role in the emergence of gender differences in comparison to processes that concern women. If true, it may not be advisable to invest more resources into the study of processes concerning men and into interventions that address them. By contrast, if processes concerning men play an equal role, perhaps more attention should be devoted to them. Hence, we conducted 3 studies on people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation that simultaneously consider processes that concern both women and men.

A “belief is (…) the mental state or function of cognising reality” (James 1889, p. 321; see Gilbert 1991). Thus, beliefs capture what people think is true (Gilbert 1991; Markóczy 1997). Beliefs can have informational value: Social role theory holds that “gender role beliefs (…) are not arbitrary or random” (Wood and Eagly 2010, p. 633), but result from observations of women’s and men’s activities in society (Eagly and Steffen 1984; Koenig and Eagly 2014). Hence, exploring beliefs is a useful starting point for a comprehensive examination of the causes of gender differences in negotiation. Moreover, beliefs have consequences, regardless of their accuracy: via confirmation bias (Nickerson 1998) and self-fulfilling prophecies (Ross et al. 2010), people act on their beliefs. Hence, research has begun to shed light on beliefs related to gender issues in negotiation (Kray et al. 2001; Manea et al. 2020).

People are likely to believe in processes that concern women (independent of their beliefs in processes that concern men) following everyday observations (Koenig and Eagly 2014). People, and women, in particular, report experiences involving prejudice toward women or sexism occurring as frequently as each week (Swim et al. 2001). Moreover, processes that concern women play a prominent role in research (e.g., Miller 2013; Rudman and Fairchild 2004), which is often disseminated to the public. Relatedly, processes concerning women, such as discrimination, are popular topics in the media (e.g., Auletta 2014). All of these experiences could lead people to endorse processes concerning women.

However, people may also endorse processes concerning men: People typically consider negotiating to be a “man’s game” (Bowles and Kray 2013, p. 4). Following this “masculine” perception of negotiations (Kray and Thompson 2005; Kray et al. 2001), people might believe that processes concerning men cause gender differences in negotiation. Moreover, people from various cultures (Gilmore 1990) perceive manhood as easily questioned and difficult to maintain (i.e., as “precarious”; Bosson and Vandello 2011; Vandello et al. 2008). This suggests that people may frequently observe men aiming to demonstrate their manhood. Finally, Rudman and Fairchild (2004; see also Bosson et al. 2006) found that men feared backlash more so than women. As certain processes (e.g., fear of backlash) may be more pronounced among men, people may also endorse processes concerning men.

Altogether, people likely endorse processes that concern both men and women. Yet, the relative strength of these beliefs is unclear. We aim to answer this research question (RQ; see Studies 1–3):

  • RQ 1: To what extent do people believe that processes concerning men versus women underlie gender differences in negotiation?

We also explored gender differences in beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation. Certain processes, such as discrimination, can be subtle (Jones et al. 2016). In fact, women report more experiences of sexism (Swim et al. 2001), whereas men may be more aware of the precarious nature of manhood (Weaver et al. 2010; but see Bosson et al. 2021; Vandello et al. 2008). Consequently, women, more so than men, might believe in processes that concern women, and vice versa. Yet, both women and men endorse stereotypical gender roles and react with backlash to role deviations (Eagly et al. 2020; Rudman and Phelan 2008). Therefore, it could also be that women and men do not differ in their beliefs. In sum, we ask (see Studies 1–3):

  • RQ 2: Do women and men differ in their beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation?

Finally, we examine the potential consequences of people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation (see also Manea et al. 2020). This point is relevant because knowledge about the causes of gender differences is used to mitigate them. That is, diversity initiatives are meant to increase representation in leadership positions and pay equity (Kalev et al. 2006; Leslie 2019). Diversity initiatives comprise different forms, such as training decision-makers to avoid discrimination or enhancing specific skills, such as negotiation, for the target group (Leslie 2019).

Yet, diversity initiatives often are not implemented well and are not necessarily supported by all stakeholders (Berdahl et al. 2018; Nishii et al. 2018). Perhaps people only perceive a certain diversity initiative as effective, and are willing to support it, to the extent that it addresses what is perceived as the cause of the particular inequity. That is, people might view an initiative that targets processes concerning women (or men) as effective and be willing to support it only to the extent to which they believe that processes concerning women (or men) underlie gender inequities. This notion is relevant because progress toward gender parity likely is hindered if people do not deem a relevant diversity initiative as effective and support it. Thus, we ask (see Study 3):

  • RQ 3: Are people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation related to their perceived effectiveness of and willingness to support different diversity initiatives?

We conducted 3 studies to answer our RQs. Studies 1 and 2 examine RQs 1 and 2, whereas Study 3 examines all 3 RQs. We always report our sample sizes, exclusions, measures, and manipulations (Simmons et al. 2012).

2 Study 1

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Overview of Design

Study 1 is a quantitative, exploratory study focusing on RQs 1 and 2. We also designed Study 1 to explore the generalizability of the observed findings: first, negotiation research is typically conducted with students recruited in university contexts (e.g., Hüffmeier et al. 2011; Kennedy and Kray 2015), which may limit its generalizability. Thus, half of our participants were recruited on the campus of a major German university (n = 100), whereas another half (n = 100) were recruited off campus (e.g., in public places; a minority of the second half were also acquaintances of the experimenter). Second, we recruited 50 men and 50 women on campus, and also 50 women and 50 men off campus. Third, we built two survey versions to address potential order effects. Fourth, we examined the influence of negotiation experience (Mazei et al. 2015). Finally, we asked participants to indicate which gender is more successful at negotiating because people might differ in their beliefs about causes depending on their general perceptions of gender differences.

2.1.2 Sample

We aimed to recruit 200 participants, and 200 participants ultimately took part in our study. A sensitivity power analysis (two-tailed, α & β = 0.05) performed with G*Power (Faul et al. 2007) based on a design examining differences between two dependent means revealed that effect sizes as small as dz = 0.26 can be detected with this sample size. One male participant who gave an answer that suggested he was not taking the study seriously was excluded from all analyses. The average age of the included participants was M = 30.36 years (SD = 12.56; range: 18–67; one participant did not indicate his age). The sample consisted of 81 students, 71 working participants, 10 participants indicating other occupational activities, and 37 participants indicating multiple occupational activities. Of the 71 working participants, 11 described their work as realistic, one as investigative, four as artistic, 22 as social, 13 as enterprising, and six as conventional (see Holland 1996); 14 participants indicated multiple characterizations.

2.1.3 Procedure

Participants were asked to fill out a paper–pencil survey that can be retrieved here: https://osf.io/ykfs2/?view_only=9136049f14304525808f9cea36b8eed3 (the data are also available as per this link). Participants read that research has observed gender differences in negotiations, yet that their causes are not yet well understood, which is why we asked participants for their assessment.

2.1.4 First Measure on People’s Beliefs about the Causes of Gender Differences

Participants then indicated on a scale ranging from 1 (do not agree at all; or strongly disagree, in the English version of this measure as used in Study 3) to 7 (strongly agree) their beliefs regarding six processes. The processes are shown in Table 1, along with the orders in which they were presented depending on the survey version. The processes were preceded by the statement: “Gender differences in negotiations emerge because …”.

Table 1 People’s Beliefs about Causes of Gender Differences in Negotiation, Presentation Orders (for Study 1), and Descriptive Statistics (Studies 1 and 3)

Three processes, chosen on the basis of their central role in past work (see Table 1), concerned women. Although all 3 processes concerned women, they differed in content and the perspective that is taken (i.e., women’s feelings and perceptions as compared to other people’s expectations regarding women; see Rudman and Fairchild 2004). Moreover, 3 processes concerned men, which again reflect key notions in extant work (e.g., Kennedy and Kray 2015; Mazei et al. 2021; see Table 1). Altogether, the first measure followed a within-subjects design with one factor and six levels. Within-subjects designs provide rich context information as participants can directly compare different stimuli with each other, and, thus, these designs are “useful in terms of uncovering judgment processes of a single individual” (Aguinis and Bradley 2014, p. 361).

There were 3 missing values in participants’ answers to this measure. The pattern of missing values did not differ from missing completely at random, χ2(9) = 5.01, p = 0.833. The missing values were replaced with the mean response across all six processes (M = 4.15). As a sensitivity analysis, we performed a Friedman’s ANOVA (see Results section) while excluding cases with missing values. The same effects emerged in this sensitivity analysis. Thus, to avoid losing any data, we report analyses in which missing values were replaced below.

2.1.5 Second Measure on People’s Beliefs about the Causes of Gender Differences

Participants indicated on a bipolar scale how relevant they believed processes that concern women to be, generally speaking, directly in comparison to processes that concern men for the emergence of gender differences. Participants provided answers on a 7-point scale that was anchored with “Men. It is due to their behavior and the expectations toward them” and with “Women. It is due to their behavior and the expectations toward them,” with a middle option of “It is due to both genders equally” (which anchor was provided on the left hand side again depended on the survey version). In the analyses, lower values on this measure indicate stronger beliefs in processes that concern men.

Three participants provided unclear responses to this item (e.g., they marked the scale twice, at two different places). These responses were replaced with the mean response to this item (M = 3.83). As a sensitivity analysis, we examined whether a one sample t-test (see Results section) yielded different results when these cases were excluded. However, the results were again robust. Thus, we again report analyses in which these responses were replaced.

2.1.6 Demographics

Participants indicated their demographics and noted whether German was their first language (respectively, whether they spoke German as well as their first language; n = 195 indicated that this was the case; the other four participants were also most likely fluent in German as they were students).

2.1.7 Negotiation Experience

Participants indicated on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very) how experienced they were regarding negotiating (M = 3.84, SD = 1.55). There were two missing values, but the results for negotiation experience did not differ depending on whether missing values were replaced with the mean or whether these cases were excluded.

2.1.8 Perceived Direction of Gender Difference

Participants indicated how men and women compared as negotiators. The two response options were “women negotiate worse than men” (as chosen by 140 participants) and “men negotiate worse than women” (as chosen by 45 participants; the order of the response options again depended on the survey version). Fourteen participants did not provide an answer to this item at all or provided an ambiguous answer, but the results for this factor did not differ depending on whether these participants were excluded or not. Thus, again to avoid losing any data, we treated this item as a factor with 3 levels (i.e., women negotiate worse than men vs. men negotiate worse than women vs. missing/ambiguous). Please note that, in the introduction to the study, we did not provide a verbal comparison of women and men, for instance, in the form of “women negotiate worse than men.” Doing so might have introduced a linguistic framing effect (Bruckmüller et al. 2012; Miller et al. 1991), which could have influenced the results.

2.2 Results

2.2.1 First Measure

The within-subjects factor, denoted as “process,” is key to examine RQ 1, asking how much people believe in processes concerning men versus women. Table 1 depicts descriptive statistics. Our study also included the between-subjects factors of recruitment location, survey version, and participant gender (see RQ 2, asking whether women and men differ in their beliefs). Moreover, we measured people’s negotiation experience (a covariate) and their perceptions of the direction of gender differences. Thus, we first conducted a mixed ANCOVA including all of these predictors. These results should be interpreted with caution, however, because assumptions for conducting parametric tests were not met. Given that the assumption of sphericity was not met, we report Huynh–Feldt-corrected statistics. The mixed ANCOVA only revealed a main effect for the process–factor, F(4.97, 874.84) = 7.80, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.04, but no other effects (all Fs ≤ 2.67, all ps ≥ 0.104).

Hence, we next conducted a Friedman’s ANOVA as a robust analysis (Field 2013). The Friedman’s ANOVA again revealed significant differences among the six processes, χ2(5) = 244.22, p < 0.001. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni-corrected p values revealed that all 3 processes that concerned women did not differ from each other (all zs ≤|1.06|, all ps = 1.00, ns), just like all 3 processes that concerned men did not differ from each other (all zs ≤|2.21|, all ps ≥ 0.406, ns). However, people endorsed more strongly each process concerning men in comparison to each process concerning women. Table 2 provides inferential statistics and effect sizes. The average effect size was large (Cumming 2014).

Table 2 Inferential Statistics and Effect Sizes (Studies 1 and 3)

2.2.2 Second Measure

To examine RQ 1, we compared the mean response on the bipolar scale to its midpoint (i.e., 4). A non-significant result would indicate the belief that gender differences are due to women and men equally. A Shapiro–Wilk test suggested that the assumption of normality was not met. Thus, we conducted a one-sample t-test with bootstrapping (bias corrected and accelerated; 5000 samples). The mean response, M = 3.83 (SD = 0.96), was lower than the midpoint of the scale, t(198) = − 2.48, bootstrapped p = 0.016, 95% BCa CI [− 0.30, − 0.03]. Although this finding was significant, suggesting that people endorsed processes concerning men, the mean response was just 0.17 points below the midpoint. Thus, the practical relevance of this finding should not be overstated, and a conservative interpretation would be that people tend towards the belief that gender differences are due to processes that concern both women and men.

We next conducted an ANCOVA to explore whether responses to the second measure were influenced by participant gender (see RQ 2), along with the survey version, recruitment location, negotiation experience, and people’s answers to the item asking how women and men compare as negotiators. The analysis did not reveal any effects for gender or the other variables (all Fs ≤ 3.58, all ps ≥ 0.060, ns), thereby not suggesting gender differences in beliefs (see RQ 2).

2.3 Discussion

Both women and men more strongly endorsed processes underlying gender differences in negotiation that concern men (see RQs 1 and 2). This does not mean, however, that people rejected processes that concern women: As shown in Table 1, people believed in processes that concern women to a moderate degree (see also the results on our second measure on people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences). Hence, altogether, the key finding of Study 1 is that participants endorsed processes that concern both women and men, which is contrary to the tendency in extant research to mainly examine processes concerning women. Study 1 is limited, however, as participants responded to a predetermined set of certain processes.

3 Study 2

To address the aforementioned limitation, we conducted Study 2 that utilized an open-ended, qualitative method. We preregistered the expectation that, although some participants would mention processes concerning women as underlying gender differences in negotiation, other participants would mention processes concerning men. In other words, we expected that people would mention processes concerning both women and men. This expectation was based on Study 1 and research by Kray et al. (2001): they asked participants to write an essay about whether and why they think one gender is more successful in negotiation. Participants typically believed that men are more successful, and the most common reason was that “men do not want to lose to women” (Kray et al. 2001, p. 958). However, this study included only a rather small and selective sample of 50 MBA students, most of them being men, which may have increased the likelihood of observing an endorsement of processes that concern men. Similarly, participants were asked which gender “has the distributive advantage” (Kray et al. 2001, p. 944), which again might have led participants to mention processes that concern men, as men typically do have the economic “advantage” (e.g., Stuhlmacher and Walters 1999). These limitations highlight the need for additional research.

Our preregistration, study materials, and data for Study 2 can be retrieved here: https://osf.io/3ythj/?view_only=6444338886af4847828185e50bf19f1e; and here https://osf.io/5jgva/?view_only=598cda63ab224a4e9ece7925127d6d31.

3.1 Method

3.1.1 Sample

We aimed to recruit 100 German participants via an online panel (i.e., Prolific) to take part in a study about “beliefs on the topic of gender and negotiation,” for which they received £0.75 as remuneration. The panel appeared to have invited slightly more participants, as our study was accessed 115 times. Following our preregistration, we excluded 13 participants who accessed the study, but later did not consent to the use of their data.

The average age of the N = 102 participants was M = 29.58 years (SD = 10.93; range: 18–68). The sample consisted of 37 women, 64 men, and one participant who selected “diverse” as their gender (e.g., Eddy 2018). Participants’ occupational activities were as follows: 42 college students, 3 high-school students, 43 working participants, and 14 participants indicating other occupational activities. Of the 43 working participants, four described their work as realistic, 13 as investigative, six as artistic, six as social, 10 as enterprising, and four as conventional (Holland 1996).

3.1.2 Procedure

Participants were asked: “What do you think: What is the cause, or are the causes, of gender differences in negotiation? In other words, which process, or which processes, underlie gender differences in negotiation?” Participants were asked to describe at least one process using a text box.

3.1.3 Further Measures

Participants provided information on their demographics, they noted whether German was their first language (respectively, whether they spoke German as well as their first language; n = 100 indicated that this was the case), they indicated their degree of negotiation experience (M = 3.31, SD = 1.54), and their perceptions regarding the direction of the gender difference (79 participants chose “women negotiate worse than men,” and 23 participants chose “men negotiate worse than women”).

3.1.4 Coding

One of the authors and a Master’s student conducted the coding. To make sure that both coders had the same understanding of our coding scheme, we began by coding an initial set of ten responses and then compared our coding. This initial set was not included when examining interrater reliability. The remaining responses were coded independently by the two coders.

If a response included a process that concerned women, we coded that at least one process concerning women was mentioned (κ = 1.00). Likewise, if a response included a process that concerned men, we coded that at least one process concerning men was mentioned (κ = 0.98). If a response could not be clearly categorized, for instance, as a response did not clearly refer to a particular gender but only mentioned “discrimination” or “prejudice,” we coded the response as “unclear or mixed” (κ = 0.97). In our preregistration, we also mentioned that we would code each individual process if more than one process was mentioned. However, during the coding process, it became evident that it was unclear how many processes participants meant to indicate. For instance, it was unclear at times whether participants were elaborating on the consequences of one particular process or already describing another process. This unexpected difficulty is reflected in the related low interrater reliabilities: we used a dummy-coded variable with two levels (i.e., a participant mentioned multiple processes concerning women [= 1] or not [= 0]), for which the interrater agreement was estimated at κ = 0.61; for another dummy-variable indicating whether or not multiple processes concerning men were mentioned, κ = 0.26. Thus, to remain conservative in our conclusions, we refrained from analyzing whether participants had mentioned multiple processes and exactly how many processes participants had mentioned.

Finally, we analyzed participants’ responses in a bottom-up fashion. The Master’s student began by developing and assigning codes for each response. One of the authors then also went through each response and thoroughly examined and refined the themes.

3.2 Results

Of the N = 102 participants, 64 mentioned at least one process that concerned women, and 56 mentioned at least one process that concerned men (24 participants provided unclear or mixed responses). Hence, participants mentioned not only processes that concerned women but also processes concerning men. These basic frequencies inform RQ 1 and are generally in line with our preregistered expectation that people would mention processes concerning both women and men.

We next more closely examined how many participants indicated processes that concerned women relative to processes that concerned men by creating a new variable with 3 categories, denoted as “categorization of responses”: a participant mentioned (a) only one or more processes that concerned women, (b) only one or more processes that concerned men, or (c) processes that concerned both women and men. Following our preregistration, participants who gave unclear responses were excluded from this analysis (n = 24). A goodness-of-fit test revealed that categories were not distributed equally, χ2(2) = 16.00, p < 0.001. Again, generally in line with our expectations (see above), most participants (n = 42) mentioned processes that concerned both women and men, although there were slightly more participants (n = 22) who mentioned only processes that concerned women compared to participants (n = 14) who only mentioned processes that concerned men.

Then, we explored the effects of people’s perceived direction of gender differences, negotiation experience, and gender (participants who gave unclear responses were again excluded). In line with Study 1, there were no significant relationships between categorization of responses and people’s perception of which gender is more successful at negotiating, χ2(2) = 2.99, p = 0.225, or their level of negotiation experience, χ2(2) = 0.03, p = 0.988 (for this analysis, experience was dichotomized, such that values of 1 to 3 were recoded as 0 = less experience, n = 45, and values of 4–7 were recoded as 1 = more experience, n = 33). However, there was a significant relationship between categorization of responses and participant gender, χ2(2) = 8.94, p = 0.011. Of the 32 women in this analysis, 14 mentioned only processes that concerned women, two mentioned only processes that concerned men, and 16 mentioned processes that concerned both women and men. Of the 46 men in this analysis, eight mentioned only processes that concerned women, 12 mentioned only processes that concerned men, and 26 mentioned processes that concerned both women and men. Thus, although both women and men were most likely to mention processes that concerned both women and men, they were also more likely to mention only processes that concern their own gender compared to processes concerning the other gender. A relationship with participant gender was not observed in Study 1, yet this finding informs RQ 2 and supports the notion presented in the General Introduction that people who are not subject to a process may not easily notice its presence.

Finally, we examined participants’ responses in a bottom-up fashion. The results are presented in Table 3. Given our focus on processes that specifically concern women or men, the table does not include themes that did not clearly refer to a particular gender (likewise, the table does not include themes by participants who only provided unclear responses). The first category of themes revolved around attributes and behaviors of women and men. For example, women were often described as cooperative or emotional (e.g., Eagly et al. 2020). Thus, themes emerged—such as feminine attributes or women’s fear of backlash—that are consistent with extant research. Likewise, themes regarding men emerged that mirror past research. For example, men were described as assertive or dominant (e.g., Rudman et al. 2012). Similarly, participants mentioned men’s toughness (Berke et al. 2017; Fowler and Geers 2017), that they aim to win (Kray and Haselhuhn 2012), or that they ought to be successful (Berdahl et al. 2018; Williams et al. 2016).

Table 3 Themes (Study 2)

Study 2 also yielded themes that have received relatively little attention, such as physical (see also Haselhuhn et al. 2014) or evolutionary aspects. For example, participants described that motherhood (or being pregnant) can yield problems for women (Bear and Glick 2017; Correll et al. 2007). Similarly, participants mentioned men’s physical strength or the issue of intrasexual competition among men (Lee et al. 2017). Another category of themes involved more structural issues. For example, participants mentioned caregiving duties or part-time work among women (Artz et al. 2018; Mazei and Hüffmeier 2021), as well as men’s breadwinner status (see also Bowles and McGinn 2008; Livingston 2014).

3.3 Discussion

Women and men typically mentioned processes underlying gender differences in negotiation concerning both women and men (RQ 1). Moreover, women and men slightly differed in their beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation (RQ 2). Finally, many processes were mentioned that are already well known in the literature (recall that our first measure on people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation in Study 1 was also based on extant research). Yet, additional themes emerged that have yet to be investigated in greater depth (e.g., the roles of motherhood or physical strength). Altogether, the key finding of Study 2 is again that participants endorsed not only processes that concern women, which have been mainly explored in past research, but also processes concerning men.

4 Study 3

Despite their strengths, Studies 1 and 2 did not address the practical consequences of people’s beliefs (e.g., Manea et al. 2020; Ross et al. 2010). Thus, we conducted Study 3—a quantitative, confirmatory study—to examine all 3 RQs. Based on our results from Study 1, we hypothesized that people would believe more strongly in processes that concern men relative to processes that concern women (Hypothesis 1). In line with their beliefs about causes, we hypothesized that people would believe that diversity initiatives that are based on processes that concern men are more effective (Hypothesis 2a), and that they would be willing to support them (Hypothesis 2b), relative to diversity initiatives based on processes that concern women. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the more people believe in processes that concern men as underlying gender differences in negotiation, the more they will perceive diversity initiatives based on processes that concern men as effective (Hypothesis 3a) and the more they will support them (Hypothesis 3b). Likewise, the more people believe in processes that concern women as underlying gender differences in negotiation, the more they will perceive diversity initiatives based on processes that concern women as effective (Hypothesis 4a) and the more they will support them (Hypothesis 4b).

Our preregistration, study materials, and data for Study 3 can be retrieved here: https://osf.io/pqzhx/?view_only=0b6730cf51c343b1ba39c8ce4b0a3cdb; and here: https://osf.io/kp456/?view_only=3e66a08cc1714073a71c7a8575ccd30a.

4.1 Method

4.1.1 Sample

Leslie et al. (2020) examined (among other factors) the relationships among different types of “beliefs regarding the importance of demographic differences and how to navigate them” (p. 453) and support for different diversity policies. They observed (uncorrected) correlations between r = − 0.20 and r = 0.45. As we examine a novel domain, we use the more conservative effect size of r =|.20| for our a priori power analyses that we performed with G*Power (Faul et al. 2007). The analysis yielded a needed sample size of N = 319 (two-tailed, α & β = 0.05). A sensitivity power analysis (two-tailed, α & β = 0.05) based on a design examining differences between two dependent means—as is relevant for Hypotheses 1, 2a, and 2b—revealed that effect sizes as small as dz = 0.20 can be detected with such a sample size (Study 1 revealed larger effect sizes). Finally, as we are interested in the generalizability of our results, we conducted Study 3 with participants from Germany and the United States.

Thus, we aimed to recruit 319 German participants as well as 319 U.S. participants via an online panel (i.e., Prolific) to take part in a study about “beliefs about gender, negotiation, and interventions,” for which they received £1.50 as remuneration. Participants who took part in Study 2 were not allowed to take part in Study 3. The panel again appeared to have invited slightly more participants (which we included as an expectation in our preregistration, following our experiences with Study 2). Following our preregistration, we excluded 31 participants who did not give their consent after accessing the study, nine additional participants who did not pass an attention check (see below), and one additional participant who appeared to take part twice (this criterion was not preregistered, yet we deem it a straightforward decision to exclude this participant).

The average age of the final N = 630 participants, composed of 313 participants from the U.S. and 317 participants from Germany, was M = 31.69 years (SD = 10.50; range: 18–75). The final sample consisted of 283 women, 338 men, and nine participants who selected “diverse” as their gender. Participants’ occupational activities were as follows: 281 participants were working full time, 106 were working part-time, 154 were full-time students, and 89 indicated “other” activities. Of the 387 working participants, 44 described their work as realistic, 74 as investigative, 34 as artistic, 79 as social, 78 as enterprising, and 78 as conventional (Holland 1996).

4.1.2 Procedure

Participants responded to the first measure on people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation from Study 1 (the 6 processes were presented in randomized order). Afterwards, they were shown 7 different diversity initiatives (Table 4) that were all presented with the same goal: to reduce gender differences in negotiation. Participants were asked to indicate on a scale ranging from 1 (not effective at all) to 7 (very effective) how they would evaluate, all things considered, their effectiveness (adapted from Rynes and Rosen 1995). The diversity initiatives were again grounded in extant work (e.g., Amanatullah and Morris 2010; Bowles et al. 2007; Leslie 2019). For example, research has examined negotiation strategies for women (e.g., Bear and Babcock 2017; Bowles and Babcock 2013) that can be taught in trainings. Similarly, research has examined whether men with atypical characteristics can avoid losses in their social status if alternative “proof” of their masculinity is available (Brescoll et al 2012; see also Fowler and Geers 2017), which can again be covered in trainings for men (see also Berdahl et al. 2018 on “masculine contest cultures”). Generally, diversity initiatives typically concern trainings aimed at a target group or at informing participants about prejudice and discrimination (see the initiatives for HR managers in Table 4; Leslie 2019). For exploratory reasons, we included another diversity initiative that did not refer to a particular gender—making salary ranges public—given the known relevance of pay transparency (e.g., Bowles et al. 2005; Recalde and Vesterlund 2020). The diversity initiatives were presented in randomized order, except for the one on salary ranges, which was always presented last.

Table 4 Diversity Initiatives and Descriptive Statistics (Study 3)

Afterwards, participants were asked to imagine that the interventions could be adopted in an organization that they work for, and to indicate how strongly they would support each of the seven diversity initiatives, for example, by personally investing time and energy for their development, announcement, and implementation, using a scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very strongly).

4.1.3 Further Measures

Participants provided information on their demographics. They also noted whether German (for German participants) or English (for U.S. participants) was their first language (respectively, whether they spoke German, or English, as well as their first language; n = 625 indicated that this was the case), they indicated their degree of negotiation experience (M = 3.45, SD = 1.59), they responded to an attention check asking them to simply choose a certain response option, and they indicated their perceptions regarding the direction of the gender difference (n = 479 participants chose “women negotiate worse than men, and n = 151 participants chose “men negotiate worse than women”). Findings related to people’s perceptions regarding the direction of the gender difference should be interpreted with caution because multiple participants stated, for instance, that they would have liked to have a third response option indicating that women and men negotiate equally well. Finally, participants were allowed to leave comments about the study.

4.2 Results

4.2.1 Hypothesis 1

Following our preregistration, we conducted a Friedman’s ANOVA and pairwise comparisons as follow-up tests, as assumptions for conducting parametric tests were not met. We also preregistered this type of analysis because Study 1 that also used this measure only suggested a main effect for the process–factor. The Friedman’s ANOVA revealed significant differences among the six processes, χ2(5) = 126.42, p < 0.001. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni-corrected p values revealed that all 3 processes that concerned women did not differ from each other (all zs ≤|1.24|, all ps = 1.00, ns), just like all 3 processes that concerned men did not differ from each other (all zs ≤|0.35|, all ps = 1.00, ns). However, replicating Study 1, and in line with Hypothesis 1, all 3 processes that concerned women significantly differed from all 3 processes that concerned men. Across all participants, people more strongly endorsed processes concerning men (Table 1). Table 2 provides inferential statistics and effect sizes. The average effect size was small (Cumming 2014).

Next, we conducted a mixed ANCOVA to explore the potential influences of people’s (a) gender, (b) degree of negotiation experience (a covariate), (c) perception of the direction of gender differences, and (d) cultural background (Germany vs. U.S.). Given that Study 1 only suggested a main effect for the process–factor, it is an open question whether these aspects influence our findings. Thus, in our preregistration, we simply raised an exploratory question asking about the generalizability of our findings. This analysis again needs to be interpreted with caution, as assumptions for conducting parametric tests were not met (we also again report Huynh–Feldt-corrected statistics). Finally, we excluded participants who selected “diverse” as their gender for all following analyses in this section. This was done to prevent any undue overgeneralizations about this group based on the small currently available sample (n = 9).

The mixed ANCOVA revealed main effects for participant gender, F(1, 612) = 17.26, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.03, cultural background, F(1, 612) = 9.10, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.02, and negotiation experience (included as a covariate; see Study 1), F(1, 612) = 5.41, p = 0.020, ηp2 = 0.01. Unlike Study 1 and the Friedman’s ANOVA, there was no main effect for the process–factor, F(4.15, 2540.66) = 1.60, p = 0.170, ηp2 = 0.00, but interactions between the process–factor and (a) participant gender, F(4.15, 2540.66) = 4.84, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.01, (b) cultural background, F(4.15, 2540.66) = 4.86, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.01, (c) the perception of the direction of gender differences, F(4.15, 2540.66) = 5.29, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.01, and (d) experience, F(4.15, 2540.66) = 3.16, p = 0.012, ηp2 = 0.01 (there was another interaction between participant gender and the perception of the direction of gender differences, F[1, 612] = 5.68, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.01). Such interactions were not observed in Study 1, potentially due to less statistical power. No other effects were significant (all Fs ≤ 2.87, all ps ≥ 0.054).

To visualize the interaction between the process–factor and participant gender (see RQ 2), Fig. 1 (N = 621; error bars indicate 95% CIs) shows estimated marginal means following from the mixed ANCOVA (the additional interactions involving the process–factor can be found in the Online Supplement). Friedman’s ANOVAs, conducted separately for the different subgroups, are given underneath each figure (also those in the Online Supplement). Men appeared to endorse processes concerning women to a relatively low extent (see the left hand side of Fig. 1), whereas there was largely a consensus between men and women with respect to their beliefs regarding the importance of processes concerning men (see the right hand side of Fig. 1). Altogether, processes concerning men were endorsed at least as strongly as processes concerning women, if not more so, thereby supporting Hypothesis 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Beliefs about the Causes of Gender Differences by Participant Gender (Study 3). Note: There were significant differences among the six processes for female participants (n = 283), χ2(5) = 27.84, p < 0.001, and for male participants (n = 338), χ2(5) = 167.93, p < 0.001

4.2.2 Hypotheses 2a and 2b

Following our preregistration, we tested Hypotheses 2a and 2b again by conducting Friedman’s ANOVAs. There were significant differences among the seven diversity initiatives regarding their perceived effectiveness, χ2(6) = 748.20, p < 0.001, and also regarding people’s willingness to support them, χ2(6) = 667.99, p < 0.001. However, as shown in Table 4, people typically perceived diversity initiatives focusing on men as less effective, and they were less willing to support them, than the corresponding diversity initiatives focusing on women (see, for example, the lower means for the negotiation training for men as compared to the negotiation training for women). Thus, as Hypotheses 2a and 2b clearly were not supported, we do not report further exploratory analyses for the sake of parsimony.

4.2.3 Hypotheses 3a, 3b, 4a, and 4b

As preregistered, we examined the correlations between people’s beliefs regarding each of the causes of gender differences in negotiation on the one hand and people’s perceptions regarding the effectiveness of the diversity initiatives, as well as their willingness to support them, on the other hand. As is shown in Table 5, the more people believed in causes that concerned men, the more they perceived diversity initiatives focusing on men as effective. They also expressed greater willingness to support these initiatives. Likewise, the more people believed in causes that concerned women, the more they perceived diversity initiatives focusing on women as effective. They also expressed greater willingness to support these initiatives. Thus, Hypotheses 3a, 3b, 4a, and 4b were fully supported.

Table 5 Correlations (Study 3)

4.3 Discussion

In line with Studies 1 and 2, a key finding of Study 3 is that people endorsed not only processes that concern women, but also processes that concern men (in some cases, processes concerning men were endorsed even more). This finding again suggests the potential relevance of processes concerning men, which played a smaller role in past research in comparison to processes concerning women. However, counter to our related hypotheses, people neither perceived diversity initiatives focusing on men as more effective, nor were they more willing to support them, in comparison to diversity initiatives focusing on women. This could be due to the fact that diversity initiatives that focus on women are more common in the workplace. For instance, diversity trainings often focus on a target group (e.g., women; Leslie 2019). Nevertheless, Study 3 revealed that people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiations were related to people’s perceptions of the effectiveness of different diversity initiatives and their willingness to support them.

5 General Discussion

5.1 Implications for Theory and Research

Across 3 studies, our key insight is that people endorsed processes underlying gender differences in negotiation that concern both women and men, whereas past research has mostly explored processes that concern women. For instance, in Study 2, people most frequently mentioned processes that concern both women and men (see also Kray et al. 2001). In Studies 1 and 3, at least some people even more strongly endorsed processes that concern men. Our key insight suggests the potential importance of processes concerning men for the emergence of gender differences in negotiation (e.g., Miller 2013; Lee et al. 2017). Hence, delving deeper into processes underlying gender differences in negotiation that concern men is an important avenue for negotiation research.

Our research has important theoretical implications. Men negotiate assertively (Netchaeva et al. 2015) or even unethically as they are “acutely motivated to win” (Kray and Haselhuhn 2012, p. 1124)—notions that Mazei et al. (2021) integrated in their model. Notably, we observed that people’s belief system was in line with these notions. For instance, people clearly endorsed processes such as “men try to win” (Kray and Haselhuhn 2012) or that they “want to be seen as ‘real’ men” (see Table 1; Vandello et al. 2008). As people’s beliefs are informed by their everyday observations (Koenig and Eagly 2014), our research supports extant work on masculinity and negotiation. Furthermore, although Hypotheses 2a and 2b were not supported, people’s beliefs were related to perceptions of the effectiveness of different diversity initiatives, as well as people’s willingness to support them. This is a noteworthy insight because it is known that diversity initiatives often are not implemented well (e.g., Nishii et al. 2018), which hampers progress toward gender equity (see also Berdahl et al. 2018).

An interesting question that arises from our findings is why people’s beliefs do not mirror the past focus on gender in negotiation research. Given that women tend to be disadvantaged in negotiations, it is logical that researchers have focused on what processes hinder women from negotiating successfully. However, as many people perceive manhood as being a “precarious” status (e.g., Bosson et al. 2021; Gilmore 1990), people likely have rich experiences with men aiming to underscore their manhood, also in negotiations (Kray and Haselhuhn 2012; Mazei et al. 2021). Hence, future research could examine in greater depth the pressure that men may experience to negotiate successfully. Men perceive their negotiation performance as affecting their manhood status (Kennedy and Kray 2015; Kray and Haselhuhn 2012). Being unsuccessful in negotiations, then, could create a “double burden” for men, such that they incur not only tangible losses (e.g., a lower than aspired salary) but also losses in their manhood status (e.g., Mazei et al. 2021; Miller 2013).

We also explored whether women and men differ in their beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation (RQ 2). Studies 2 and 3 point to slight differences between the genders. Specifically, in Study 2, women and men were more likely to mention only processes that concern their own gender compared to processes concerning the other gender (see also Swim et al. 2001; Weaver et al. 2010). Similarly, in Study 3, men as compared to women appeared to endorse processes concerning women slightly less. Altogether, our findings support the notion that people may be especially aware of processes that concern their own gender (Swim et al. 2001; Weaver et al. 2010; but see also Bosson et al. 2021).

Our work may also be relevant for research on the phenomenon termed the “effect to be explained” (Miller et al. 1991), which notes that people tend to highlight the group that appears atypical or lower in social status in their explanations of group differences (e.g., Bruckmüller et al. 2012). Following this phenomenon, it is surprising that people did not endorse processes that concern women to a greater extent. Thus, an interesting avenue for future research is to examine in greater depth the potentially manifold roots of people’s beliefs. Finally, recall that Study 2 revealed processes that have yet received only scant attention in research on gender and negotiation, including evolutionary and structural factors, which future work could also investigate in greater depth.

5.2 Implications for Practice

Women typically earn less than men (Blau and Kahn 2017) and hold fewer top leadership positions (Eagly and Carli 2007)—inequities that research on gender and negotiation aims to address (e.g., Kennedy and Kray 2015; Kulik and Olekalns 2012). Our research highlights that gender differences in negotiation may have multiple causes that relate to both women and men (see also Kennedy and Kray 2015; Kray et al. 2001). Thus, for the successful reduction of gender inequities, it would be helpful to examine and counteract each of these causes (e.g., in the form of diversity initiatives). However, diversity initiatives do not always achieve their goals (Nishii et al. 2018). Our results also speak to this issue: the more people believed in a certain cause of gender differences in negotiation, the more willing they were to support certain diversity initiatives. Thus, people’s beliefs about the causes of gender differences in negotiation, especially among those responsible for the implementation of diversity initiatives (e.g., Leslie 2019), should be taken into account.

5.3 Limitations and Routes for Future Research

In Studies 1 and 3, we measured different constructs with single items. Although this procedure can reduce fatigue among participants, especially when several processes and diversity initiatives have to be assessed, and although our findings appeared to be robust, future research could develop scales to measure these constructs. Moreover, future research is needed to examine the consequences of people’s beliefs in longitudinal designs as well as additional diversity initiatives (see also Manea et al. 2020; Meussen et al. 2020). Finally, although Study 3 included participants from Germany and the United States, future research is needed to examine more broadly the potential influence of people’s culture (Shan et al. 2016; 2019).

6 Conclusion

If people read a typical contemporary article on gender differences in negotiation, they are likely to learn that processes relevant to women are the key to explaining gender differences. Although these processes certainly play a crucial role, our research suggests a more nuanced story: processes that concern men and masculinity (e.g., Kray and Haselhuhn 2012; Netchaeva et al. 2015) are likely to play an important role as well. We hope that our results stimulate future research to shed light on the full range of processes underlying gender differences in negotiation.