Skip to main content
Log in

Trait perceptions influence economic out-group bias: lab and field evidence from Vietnam

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Group favoritism is typically directed toward in-group members and against out-group members, but these cross-group effects often vary. Little is known about why group effects on economic choices vary. We use a survey method developed in social psychology to measure stereotyped attitudes of one group toward another. These attitudes are then associated with prosociality in five experimental games (also using an unusual amount of individual-level sociodemographic control). We present evidence from an artificial field experiment of a majority group with high status (Vietnamese) exhibiting no disfavoritism toward a lower-status out-group (Khmer) and typical disfavoritism to a second out-group (Chinese). Both Vietnamese and Chinese groups see the Khmer as warm but incompetent, attitudes which seem to activate empathy rather than contempt. The results suggest that measuring between-group stereotype attitudes can be used to predict the sign of cross-group favoritism in other natural settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Social preference theories and measures have also influenced cognitive neuroscience (e.g., Fehr and Camerer (2007), Tricomi et al. (2010) and Ruff and Fehr (2014)).

  2. See also Ball et al. (2001), Goette et al. (2006), Charness et al. (2007), Chen and Li (2009) and Abbink et al. (2010).

  3. See Sobel (2002), Akerlof and Kranton (2005), Croson and Gneezy (2009) and Fehr et al. (2009) for reviews.

  4. Friesen et al. (2012) report outgroup favoritism by Canadian children of East Asian ethnicity toward White and South Asian children. Bauer et al. (2012) report partial outgroup favouritism among children (in an envy game, but not in a sharing game) in Georgia and Sierra Leone who were unaffected by wars in those areas.

  5. Sociodemographic controls in earlier studies included race and survey measures related to trust (Glaeser et al. 2000), gender (Fershtman and Gneezy 2001), age and gender (Burns 2006; van der Merwe and Burns 2008), measures of land, house type and education (Fehr et al. 2008; Hoff et al. 2011). The most extensive set of controls—age, gender, income and education—are in Buchan et al. (2011). Note that age and gender are useful controls for many reasons, but since they do not covary much with group ethnicity they are not a control for whether group effects are due to other variables. Our study includes all these measures (except for land, though it is highly correlated with wealth in most of the world, which we include) along with other measures. Bahry and Wilson (2004) use gender and age individual controls in comparing behavior among Tatarstan and Sakha (former USSR republics), as well as (experimental) group level controls for other variables. They find no group effects on behavior in trust games, which is a reminder that including more controls could conceivably account for part or all of the apparent group differences shown in other studies with fewer or no controls. Chen et al. (2014) use the novel approach (in experimental economics) of priming of ethnicity, finding effects in coordination and PD games.

  6. Note also that ethnicity is clearly established (i.e., subjects self-report confidently).

  7. Battle-of-the-sexes games results are not reported in this paper.

  8. Yamagishi et al. (2013) conducted an experiment with similar games to our study, spread over three and a half years, and found consistency within individuals in pro-social across different games.

  9. In VK1 and VK8 villages, Vietnamese and Khmer subjects were divided into white and red groups, respectively. In villages VK 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and 7, we divided subjects into four groups by ethnicity and income. High-income Vietnamese, low-income Vietnamese, high-income Khmer, and low-income Khmer were assigned white, black, red and pink ID tags, respectively. The instruction said the colors of their identification tags were based on ethnicity and income. In each game, subjects were first asked their decisions if their opponents were either white or black groups (Vietnamese), and red or pink groups (Khmer). Afterward, they were asked their choices contingent on each of the four color groups. Similarly, in VC villages, subjects were divided into four groups, white, black, red and pink (high-income Vietnamese, low-income Vietnamese, high-income Chinese, and low-income Chinese, respectively). The subjects were first asked their decisions for each game if their opponents were either white or black groups (Vietnamese), then if their opponents were either red or pink groups (Chinese). In this paper, we report the effects of ethnicity on decisions in games and leave the analysis of ethnicity and income effects for future analysis.

  10. We prepared Vietnamese instruction for Vietnamese and Chinese subjects, and Khmer instruction for Khmer subjects. Vietnamese instruction was provided to Khmer subjects upon request.

  11. Our longer working paper discusses measures and possible effects of social distance and political power (see Tanaka and Camerer 2012). We exclude these analyses in this paper because there were no large robust effects (as also found by Bahry and Wilson (2004) in former USSR countries).

  12. Brañas-Garza (2006) finds when subjects know that receivers are poor, they send significantly higher amounts. Fershtman and Gneezy (2001) conducted dictator games with two major ethnic groups in Israel and found no significant in-group bias.

  13. The same is true for trust games (see Table A.6).

  14. Bernhard et al. (2006), Fehr and Fischbacher (2004), Goette et al. (2006), Hoff et al. (2011).

  15. Glaeser et al. (2000) find an ingroup effect on trustee money repaid. Bouckaert and Dhaene (2004) find no ingroup effects among Belgian businessmen. In a protocol with partner choice Bornhorst et al. (2010) find no ingroup favoritism but Northern Europeans cultivate better reputations and therefore earn more than Southerners. Buchan et al. (2011) find that public good contributions to a cross-country “world group” depends on a measure of global social identity (controlling for four key sociodemographic variables).

  16. The “neighbor” question is used in the World Values Survey.

  17. For example, Todorov et al. (2005) famously found that competence judged from politicians’ headshot photos was strongly correlated with their vote shares in actual elections.

  18. The “neighbor” question is used in the World Values Survey.

  19. For example, Todorov et al. (2005) famously found that competence judged from politicians’ headshot photos was strongly correlated with their vote shares in actual elections.

References

  • Abbink, K., Brandts, J., Herrmann, B., & Orzen, H. (2010). Intergroup conflict and intra-group punishment in an experimental contest game. American Economic Review, 100(1), 420–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akerlof, G. A., & Kranton, R. E. (2000). Economics and identity. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 715–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akerlof, G. A., & Kranton, R. E. (2005). Identity and the economics of organizations. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(1), 9–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akerlof, G. A., & Kranton, R. E. (2010). Identity economics: How our identities shape our work, wages, and well-being. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ashraf, N., Bohnet, I., & Piankov, N. (2006). Decomposing trust and trustworthiness. Experimental Economics, 9(3), 193–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahry, D., & Wilson, R. K. (2004). Trust in transitional societies: Experimental results from Russia. Chicago: American Political Science Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, S., Eckel, C., Grossman, P. J., & Zame, W. (2001). Status in markets. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 161–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilová, J., & Henrich, J. (2012). Warfare experience during ontogeny increases egalitarian and parochial motivations (pp. 1–58). Maastricht: United Nations University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442(7105), 912–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94(4), 991–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornhorst, F., Ichino, A., Kirchkamp, O., Schlag, K. H., & Winter, E. (2010). Similarities and differences when building trust: the role of cultures. Experimental Economics, 13(3), 260–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouckaert, J., & Dhaene, G. (2004). Inter-ethnic trust and reciprocity: Results of an experiment with small businessmen. European Journal of Political Economy, 20(4), 869–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brañas-Garza, P. (2006). Poverty in dictator games: Awakening solidarity. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 60(3), 306–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchan, N. R., Brewer, M. B., Grimalda, G., Wilson, R. K., Fatas, E., & Foddy, M. (2011). Global social identity and global cooperation. Psychological Science, 22(6), 821–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, J. (2006). Racial stereotypes, stigma and trust in post-apartheid South Africa. Economic Modelling, 23(5), 805–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., & Weigelt, K. (1988). Experimental tests of a sequential equilibrium reputation model. Econometrica, 56(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, M. R., & Castillo, M. (2003). The economic impacts of altruism, trust and reciprocity: An experimental approach to social capital. http://eml.berkeley.edu/~webfac/bardhan/e271_sp03/carter.pdf.

  • Charness, G., & Grosskopf, B. (2001). Relative payoffs and happiness: an experimental study. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 45(3), 301–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Rabin, M. (2002). Understanding social preferences with simple tests. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(3), 817–869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., Rigotti, L., & Rustichini, A. (2007). Individual behavior and group membership. American Economic Review, 24, 1340–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y., & Li, S. X. (2009). Group identity and social preferences. American Economic Review, 99, 431–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Yan, Li, S. X., Liu, T. X., & Shih, M. (2014). Which hat to wear? Impact of natural identities on coordination and cooperation. Games and Economic Behavior, 29, 58–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, D. M., Glaeser, E. L., & Vigdor, J. L. (1999). The rise and decline of the American ghetto. Journal of Political Economy, 107(3), 455–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darity, W. A., & Mason, P. L. (1998). Evidence on discrimination in employment: Codes of color, codes of gender. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(2), 63–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Fatas, E., & Wilson, R. (2010). Cooperation and status in organizations. Journal of Public Economic Theory, 12(4), 737–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2007). Social learning in coordination games: does status matter? Experimental Economics, 10(3), 317–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Camerer, C. F. (2007). Social neuroeconomics: The neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 419–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Goette, L., & Zehnder, C. (2009). A behavioral account of the labor market: The role of fairness concerns. Annual Review of Economics, 1(1), 355–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Hoff, K., & Kshetramade, M. (2008). Spite and development. The American Economic Review, 98(2), 494–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fershtman, C., & Gneezy, U. (2001). Discrimination in a segmented society: An experimental approach. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 351–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth, then competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, J., Arifovic, J., Wright, S. C., Ludwig, A., Giamo, L., & Baray, G. (2012). Ethnic identity and discrimination among children. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(6), 1156–1169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaeser, E. L., Laibson, D. I., Scheinkman, J. A., & Soutter, C. L. (2000). Measuring trust. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 811–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goette, L., Huffman, D., & Meier, S. (2006). The impact of group membership on cooperation and norm enforcement: Evidence using random assignment to real social groups. American Economic Review, 96(2), 212–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2009). Cultural biases in economic exchange? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124(3), 1095–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habyarimana, J., Humphreys, M., Posner, D., & Weinstein, J. (2006). Social focal points. Georgetown University. http://web.mit.edu/posner/www/papers/social%20focal%20points,%20Mar%2005.pdf.

  • Hoff, K., Kshetramade, M., & Fehr, E. (2011). Caste and punishment: The legacy of caste culture in norm enforcement. The Economic Journal, 121, F449–F475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holm, H. J., & Danielson, A. (2005). Tropic trust versus nordic trust: Experimental evidence from tanzania and sweden. Economic Journal, 115(503), 505–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kumru, C., & Vesterlund, L. (2010). The effect of status on voluntary contribution. Journal of Public Economic Theory, 12(4), 709–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phung, D. T., & Phong, N. (2006). Vietnam household living standard survey (VHLSS), 2002 and 2004: Basic information. Hanoi: General Statistics Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(37), 14824–14829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, C., & Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(8), 549–562.

  • Sobel, J. (2002). Can we trust social capital? Journal of Economic Literature, 40(1), 139–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223(5), 96–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, T. & Camerer, C.F. (2012). The limits of economic in-group bias: Lab and field favoritism toward low-status Khmer in Vietnam. Working paper.

  • Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 1623–1626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tricomi, E., Rangel, A., Camerer, C. F., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Neural evidence for inequality-averse social preferences. Nature, 463(7284), 1089–1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Merwe, W. G., & Burns, J. (2008). What’s in a name? Racial identity and altruism in post-apartheid South Africa. South African Journal of Economics, 76(2), 266–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Mifune, N., Li, M., Hashimoto, H., Horita, Y., Miura, A., et al. (2013). Is behavioral pro-sociality game-specific? Pro-social preference and expectations of pro-sociality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120(2), 260–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yinger, J. (1998). Evidence on discrimination in consumer markets. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(2), 23–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The Russell Sage Foundation (TT) and Betty and Gordon Moore Foundation (CFC) supported this research. Thanks to many tireless research assistants, the audience at Stanford SITE (2009), and two referees and editor David Cooper for helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colin F. Camerer.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOC 3007 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tanaka, T., Camerer, C.F. Trait perceptions influence economic out-group bias: lab and field evidence from Vietnam. Exp Econ 19, 513–534 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-015-9452-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-015-9452-1

Keywords

Navigation