Skip to main content
Log in

Parental background and other-regarding preferences in children

  • Manuscript
  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Other-regarding preferences are important for establishing and maintaining cooperative outcomes. In this paper, we study how the formation of other-regarding preferences during childhood is related to parental background. Our subjects, aged 4–12 years, are classified into other-regarding types based on simple binary-choice dictator games. The main finding is that the children of parents with low education are less altruistic, more selfish, and more likely to be weakly spiteful. This link is robust to controlling for a rich set of children’s characteristics and class fixed effects. It also stands out against the overall development of preferences, as we find children to become more altruistic, less selfish, and less likely to be weakly spiteful with increasing age. The results, supported by a complementary analysis of World Values Survey data, suggest an important role of socialization in the formation of other-regarding preferences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Indirect evidence for childhood being a sensitive period in the development of other-regarding preferences is also provided by Bauer et al. (unpublished results) who find that exposure to warfare intensifies in-group egalitarianism, and that these effects are particularly enduring if warfare is experienced during childhood or adolescence. Researchers have also studied the development of other types of preferences: risk aversion (Eckel et al. 2011; Sutter et al. 2013), time discounting (Bettinger and Slonim 2007; Sutter et al. 2013), and trust (Harbaugh et al. 2003b; Sutter and Kocher 2007). Sutter et al. (2013) and Castillo et al. (2011) also show that experimental measures of preferences predict the field behaviour of children.

  2. While the usual practice in experiments with adult subject pools is to pay for one randomly selected task, paying for each task is common in experiments with children because it is simpler to understand. A legitimate concern with paying for each task is that children may think about the total allocations resulting from the four choices instead of considering payoffs in each individual game separately. This is unlikely in our sample, since the children made choices sequentially, they did not know how many choices were to come, and they did not know what the allocations in subsequent tasks would be. Furthermore, we tested whether different combinations of choices that result in the same total payoff for the self and for the partner were chosen with different probabilities. There are four pairs of such combinations: in two of them their frequencies are significantly different at the 1 % level, in one at the 5 % level, and in one at the 12 % level (available upon request).

  3. The design allows assessing whether other-regarding preferences vary with the familiarity and ethnicity of the recipient. Such an analysis is beyond the scope of this paper.

  4. The experiments were also approved by the Director of the Institute of Economic Studies, Faculty of Social Sciences at Charles University, to substitute for the lack of a Human Subject Review Board in the Czech university system.

  5. None of the choices in the games differs significantly between the sub-sample of children included in and excluded from the analysis (Table S4). Children whose parents filled out the questionnaire are on average younger and have better teacher evaluations of their performance compared to children whose parents did not fill the questionnaire. We perform several robustness checks (e.g. the Heckman sample selection model, see Table S5) and conclude that our results are unlikely to be affected by patterns in parental non-response.

  6. Passing a school graduation exam is considered a sign of an educated person in the Czech Republic. It is a prerequisite for applying to a college, university, or other higher education institution. Secondary education without the school graduation exam corresponds to level 3c of the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED), while secondary school with graduation exam corresponds to levels 3a and 3b.

  7. Previous research has shown that in the intergenerational transmission of personality and attitudes, the mother’s characteristics often play a more important role than the father’s characteristics (for a review see Loehlin 2008). We focus on the link with overall parental education since education levels of mothers and fathers in our sample are highly correlated and the results are robust to using the mother’s or father’s education instead of parental education (Table S7).

  8. The results are robust to using OLS and probit models (available upon request).

  9. We obtain similar results when using yet another classification into types, based on information from choices in the two costly games and disregarding choices in the costless games in which the decision-maker’s payoff is unaffected by his/her choice (Table S9). Such a classification does not allow for making combinations of choices that are inconsistent with any of the four preference types: altruistic, inequality averse, spiteful, or selfish.

  10. Interestingly, older children are also more likely to fall into the ambiguous category, suggesting that instead of behaving selfishly some older children started forming their own fairness ideals and have not yet developed a specific theory-predicted notion of fairness.

  11. Our parental questionnaire does not contain such questions, because asking them could significantly decrease the response rate.

References

  • Adriani, F., & Sonderegger, S. (2009). Why do parents socialize their children to behave pro-socially? An information-based theory. Journal of Public Economics, 93(11), 1119–1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Almås, I., Cappelen, A. W., Salvanes, K. G., Sørensen, E. O., & Tungodden, B. (2012). Willingness to compete: family matters. Norwegian School of Economics, Discussion Paper No. 24/2012.

  • Almås, I., Cappelen, A. W., Sørensen, E. O., & Tungodden, B. (2010). Fairness and the development of inequality acceptance. Science, 328(5982), 1176–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aughinbaugh, A., & Gittleman, M. (2003). Does money matter? A comparison of the effect of income on child development in the United States and Great Britain. The Journal of Human Resources, 38(2), 416–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, A., Burns, J., Miller, L., & Shaw, I. (2011). Individual notions of distributive justice and relative economic status. University of Oxford Discussion Paper No. 2011005.

  • Bartling, B., Fehr, E., & Schunk, D. (2012). Health effects on children’s willingness to compete. Experimental Economics, 15(1), 58–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilová, J., & Henrich, J. (unpublished results). Warfare experience during ontogeny increases egalitarian and parochial motivations. CERGE-EI.

  • Benenson, J. F., Pascoe, J., & Radmore, N. (2007). Children’s altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 168–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, E., & Slonim, R. (2007). Patience among children. Journal of Public Economics, 91(1–2), 343–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bisin, A., & Verdier, T. (2001). The economics of cultural transmission and the dynamics of preferences. Journal of Economic Theory, 97(2), 298–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, G. E., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). ERC: a theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. The American Economic Review, 90(1), 166–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (2006). Microeconomics: behavior, institutions, and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2002). The inheritance of inequality. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 16(3), 3–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., Gintis, H., & Osborne Groves, M. (2008). Unequal chances: family background and economic success. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carneiro, P., & Heckman, J. J. (2002). The evidence on credit constraints in post-secondary schooling. The Economic Journal, 112(482), 705–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, A., & Katz, L. F. (1991). The company you keep: the effects of family and neighborhood on disadvantaged youths. NBER Working Paper No. 3075.

  • Castillo, M., Ferraro, P., Jordan, J., & Petrie, R. (2011). The today and tomorrow of kids: time preferences and educational outcomes of children. Journal of Public Economics, 95(11–12), 1377–1385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Wallace, B. (2008). Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(10), 3721–3726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Rabin, M. (2002). Understanding social preferences with simple tests. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(3), 817–869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., Heckman, J. J., Lochner, L., & Masterov, D. V. (2006). Interpreting the evidence on life cycle skill formation. In E. Hanushek & F. Welch (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (Vol. 1, pp. 697–812). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa-Gomes, M., Crawford, V. P., & Broseta, B. (2001). Cognition and behavior in normal-form games: an experimental study. Econometrica, 69(5), 1193–1235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixit, A. (2009). Governance institutions and economic activity. The American Economic Review, 99(1), 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Grossman, P. J., Johnson, C. A., De Oliveira, A., Rojas, C., & Wilson, R. K. (2011). On the development of risk preferences: experimental evidence. CBEES Working Paper No. 2008-5.

  • Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. The American Psychologist, 59(2), 77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, A., Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2005). Driving forces behind informal sanctions. Econometrica, 73(6), 2017–2030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454(7208), 1079–1083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2002). Why social preferences matter: the impact of non-selfish motives on competition, cooperation, and incentives. The Economic Journal, 112, C1–C33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism: Proximate patterns and evolutionary origins. Nature, 425, 785–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Hoff, K. (2011). Introduction: tastes, castes and culture: the influence of society on preferences. The Economic Journal, 121(556), F396–F412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Rützler, D., & Sutter, M. (2011). The development of egalitarianism, altruism, spite and parochialism in childhood and adolescence. IZA Discussion Paper No. 5530.

  • Fong, C. M., Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2006). Strong reciprocity and the welfare state. In S. C. Kolm & J. M. Ythier (Eds.), Handbook on the economics of giving, reciprocity and altruism (pp. 1439–1464). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Krause, K., & Liday, S. J. (2003a). Bargaining by children. University of Oregon Economics Department Working Paper No. 2002-04.

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Krause, K., Liday, S. J., & Vesterlund, L. (2003b). Trust in children. In E. Ostrom & J. Walker (Eds.), Trust and reciprocity: interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research (pp. 302–322). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312(5782), 1900–1902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Gintis, H., Fehr, E., Camerer, C., McElreath, R., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Tracer, D., Marlow, F., Patton, J., Alvard, M., Gil-White, F., & Henrich, N. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(06), 795–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, B., Thoni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 1362–1367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakiela, P., Miguel, E., & TeVelde, V. L. (2010). You’ve earned it: combining field and lab experiments to estimate the impact of human capital on social preferences. NBER Working Paper No. 16449.

  • Leibbrandt, A. (2012). Are social preferences related to market performance? Experimental Economics, 15(4), 589–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (2008). Resemblance in personality and attitudes between parents and their children: genetic and environmental contributions. In S. Bowles, H. Gintis, & M. Osborne Groves (Eds.), Unequal chances: family background and economic success (pp. 192–207). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martinsson, P., Nordblom, K., Rützler, D., & Sutter, M. (2011). Social preferences during childhood and the role of gender and age: an experiment in Austria and Sweden. Economics Letters, 110(3), 248–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Public Health (2001). 6th nation-wide anthropological survey of children and adolescents 2001: Czech Republic.

  • Rigdon, M., & Levine, A. S. (unpublished results). The role of expectations and gender in altruism. Rutgers University.

  • Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S., Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime effects: the High/Scope Perry Preschool study through age 40. Ypsilanti: High/Scope Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutter, M., & Kocher, M. G. (2007). Trust and trustworthiness across different age groups. Games and Economic Behavior, 59(2), 364–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutter, M., Feri, F., Kocher, M. G., Martinsson, P., Nordblom, K., & Rützler, D. (2010). Social preferences in childhood and adolescence: a large-scale experiment. University of Gothenburg Working Papers in Economics No. 454.

  • Sutter, M., Kocher, M. G., Glätzle-Rützler, D., & Trautmann, S. (2013). Impatience and uncertainty: experimental decisions predict adolescents’ field behavior. The American Economic Review, 103(1), 510–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research was supported by a grant from the CERGE-EI Foundation under a program of the Global Development Network and by the Czech Science Foundation (P402/10/P103—Chytilová, P402/12/G130—Bauer). All opinions expressed are those of the authors and have not been endorsed by any of the supporting institutions. We thank C. Camerer, A. Cappelen, M. Chu, R. Filer, W. Harbaugh, Š. Jurajda, P. Katuščák, F. Matějka, the editor David Cooper, and two anonymous referees for valuable comments and K. Bát’a, J. Cahlíková, J. Hercher, and J. Palguta for excellent research assistance. We are grateful to the headmasters of the cooperating schools and kindergartens for their support.

The research reported in this paper is not the result of a for-pay consulting relationship, nor do the authors or their employers have a financial interest in the topic of the paper that might constitute a conflict of interest.

All authors have approved the final article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julie Chytilová.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bauer, M., Chytilová, J. & Pertold-Gebicka, B. Parental background and other-regarding preferences in children. Exp Econ 17, 24–46 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-013-9355-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-013-9355-y

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation