Skip to main content
Log in

The Scarcity of Interleaved Practice in Mathematics Textbooks

  • Commentary
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A typical mathematics assignment consists of a block of problems devoted to the same topic, yet several classroom-based randomized controlled trials have found that students obtain higher test scores when most practice problems are mixed with different kinds of problems—a format known as interleaved practice. Interleaving prevents students from safely assuming that each practice problem relates to the same skill or concept as the previous problem, thus forcing them to choose an appropriate strategy on the basis of the problem itself. Yet despite the efficacy of interleaved practice, blocked practice predominates most mathematics textbooks. As an illustration, we examined 13,505 practice problems in six representative mathematics texts and found that only 9.7% of the problems were interleaved. This translates to only one or two interleaved problems per school day. In brief, strong evidence suggests that students benefit from heavy doses of interleaved practice, yet most mathematics texts provide scarcely any.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allwood, M., Bullard, F., Chumas, M., Fritz, K., Harris, S., Humphrey, M., Kotz, B., Olsen, C., Peck, R., Sheets, K., Sukow, A., & Tate, S. (2014). SpringBoard mathematics: course 2. New York, NY: College Board.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barzagar Nazari, K., & Ebersbach, M. (2019). Distributing mathematical practice of third and seventh graders: applicability of the spacing effect in the classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 288–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J., Burger, E., Chard, D., Hall, E., Kennedy, P., Renfro, F., Roby, T., Scheer, J., & Waits, B. K. (2012). Holt McDougal mathematics: grade 7. Orlando, FL: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blazar, D., Heller, B., Kane, T., Polikoff, M., Staiger, D., Carrell, S., … & Kurlaender, M. (2019). Learning by the book: comparing math achievement growth by textbook in six common core states. Research Report. Cambridge, MA: Center for Education Policy Research, Harvard University.

  • Burger, E. B., Dixon, J. K., Kanold, T. D., Larson, M. R., Leinwand, S. J., & Sandoval-Martinez, M. E. (2014). Go math: middle school, grade 7. Orlando, FL: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Cepeda, N. J., Rohrer, D., Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Using spacing to enhance diverse forms of learning: review of recent research and implications for instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 369–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, J. A., Cuevas, G. J., Day, R., Malloy, C. E., Kersaint, G., Reynosa, M., Silbey, R., & Vielhaber, K. (2015). Glencoe math: built to the common core. Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, O., Castro-Alonso, J. C., Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2018). Extending cognitive load theory to incorporate working memory resource depletion: evidence from the spacing effect. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 483–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deans for Impact. (2015). The science of learning. Austin, TX: Deans for Impact.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driskell, J. E., Willis, R. P., & Copper, C. (1992). Effect of overlearning on retention. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 615–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J. (2013). Strengthening the student toolbox: study strategies to boost learning (pp. 12–21). Fall: American Educator.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EdReports.org (2019). Compare materials. Retrieved October 17, 2019, from www.edreports.org.

  • Foster, N. L., Mueller, M. L., Was, C., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2019). Why does interleaving improve math learning? The contributions of discriminative contrast and distributed practice. Memory & Cognition, 47(6), 1088–1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, S. (2012). Saxon math: course 2. Orlando, FL: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, R. F., Lyle, K. B., Hieb, J. L., & Ralston, P. A. (2016). Spaced retrieval practice increases college students’ short-and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 853–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, B. (2001). Implementing standards: the California mathematics textbook debacle. Phi Delta Kappan, 83(3), 264–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi. Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 248–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lappan, G., Phillips, E., Fey, J., & Friel, S. (2014). Connected mathematics, grade 7. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., & Boswell, L. (2014). Big ideas math: a common core curriculum (Red ed.). Erie, PA: Big Ideas Learning, LLC.

  • Le Blanc, K., & Simon, D. (2008). Mixed practice enhances retention and JOL accuracy for mathematical skills. Paper presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL.

  • Lyle, K. B., Bego, C. R., Hopkins, R. F., Hieb, J. L., & Ralston, P. A. S. (in press). How the amount and spacing of retrieval practice affect the short- and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge. Educational Psychology Review.

  • Mayfield, K. H., & Chase, P. N. (2002). The effects of cumulative practice on mathematics problem solving. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 105–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostrow, K., Heffernan, N., Heffernan, C., & Peterson, Z. (2015). Blocking vs. interleaving: examining single-session effects within middle school math homework. In Artificial Intelligence in Education (pp. 338–347). Cham: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: a cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, S. C. (2015). The interleaving effect: mixing it up boosts learning. Scientific American Mind.

  • Polikoff, M. (2018). The challenges of curriculum materials as a reform lever. EducationNext. Retrieved from www.educationnext.org/challenges-curriculum-materials-reform-lever

  • Rau, M. A., Aleven, V., & Rummel, N. (2013). Interleaved practice in multi-dimensional learning tasks: which dimension should we interleave? Learning and Instruction, 23, 98–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). Inexpensive techniques to improve education: applying cognitive psychology to enhance educational practice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(4), 242–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2006). The effects of overlearning and distributed practice on the retention of mathematics knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1209–1224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics practice problems boosts learning. Instructional Science, 35, 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., & Stershic, S. (2015). Interleaved practice improves mathematics learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 900–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C. N. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 40–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sana, F., Yan, V. X., & Kim, J. A. (2017). Study sequence matters for the inductive learning of cognitive concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 84–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutte, G. M., Duhon, G. J., Solomon, B. G., Poncy, B. C., Moore, K., & Story, B. (2015). A comparative analysis of massed vs. distributed practice on basic math fact fluency growth rates. Journal of School Psychology, 53(2), 149–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, K., & Rohrer, D. (2010). The effect of interleaving practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 837–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. T. (2014). Strategies that make learning last. Educational Leadership, 72(2), 10–15.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Sandra Stershic Kauffman and Chi-Ngai Cheung for their help with scoring.

Funding

The research reported here was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education, through grant R305A160263 to the University of South Florida.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Doug Rohrer.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Disclaimer

The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent the views of the US Department of Education.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R.F. & Hartwig, M.K. The Scarcity of Interleaved Practice in Mathematics Textbooks. Educ Psychol Rev 32, 873–883 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09516-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09516-2

Keywords

Navigation